Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Self-Regulation and Sleep Duration, Sleepiness, and Chronotype in Adolescents

Judith A. Owens, Tracy Dearth-Wesley, Daniel Lewin, Gerard Gioia and Robert C. Whitaker
Pediatrics November 2016, e20161406; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1406
Judith A. Owens
aDivision of Neurology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tracy Dearth-Wesley
bCenter for Obesity Research and Education and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daniel Lewin
cDivisions of Pulmonary and Sleep Medicine and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gerard Gioia
dNeuropsychology, Children’s National Medical Center, George Washington University School of Medicine, Washington, District of Columbia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robert C. Whitaker
bCenter for Obesity Research and Education and
eDepartments of Epidemiology and Biostatistics and
fPediatrics, Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: To determine whether shorter school-night sleep duration, greater daytime sleepiness, and greater eveningness chronotype were associated with lower self-regulation among adolescents.

METHODS: An online survey of 7th- to 12th-grade students in 19 schools in Fairfax County, Virginia Public Schools was conducted in 2015. Self-regulation was measured with the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, 2nd edition, Screening Self-Report Form. Sleep measures included school night-sleep duration (hours between usual bedtime and wake time), daytime sleepiness (Sleepiness Scale in the Sleep Habits Survey, tertiles), and chronotype (Morningness–Eveningness Scale for Children, continuous score and tertiles). Sociodemographic factors and mental health conditions were analyzed as potential confounders.

RESULTS: Among 2017 students surveyed, the mean age was 15.0 years (range, 12.1–18.9 years), and 21.7% slept <7 hours on school nights. In regression models adjusted for confounders, there was a significant independent association between self-regulation and both chronotype (P < .001) and daytime sleepiness (P < .001) but not sleep duration (P = .80). Compared with those in the lowest tertile of daytime sleepiness, those in the highest tertile had lower (0.59 SD units; 95% confidence interval, 0.48–0.71) self-regulation, as did those in the eveningness tertile of chronotype compared with those in the morningness tertile (0.35 SD units lower; 95% confidence interval, 0.24–0.46).

CONCLUSIONS: Among adolescents, greater daytime sleepiness and greater eveningness chronotype were independently associated with lower self-regulation, but shorter sleep duration was not. Aspects of sleep other than school-night sleep duration appear to be more strongly associated with self-regulation.

What’s Known on This Subject:

Chronic sleep loss in adolescents has been linked to impaired self-regulation and increased risk of adverse health and functional outcomes. However, less is known about how self-regulation is related to daytime sleepiness and circadian phase preference (chronotype).

What This Study Adds:

Among adolescents, daytime sleepiness and a more “eveningness” chronotype (ie, tendency for later sleep onset and offset and timing of activity patterns) are both stronger predictors of poor self-regulation than is short nighttime sleep duration.

Insufficient sleep has reached epidemic levels among adolescents in the United States1–7 and in other nations.8–12 Chronic sleep loss in teenagers is now considered a public health problem by leading advocates for children’s health, including the American Academy of Pediatrics,13 the American Medical Association,14 and the US Department of Health and Human Services,15 because of its potential adverse impacts on adolescent cardiometabolic health,16–19 mental health,20–24 substance use,25–27 safety,28–33 and academic performance.34,35

Impaired self-regulation may be 1 mechanism by which insufficient sleep leads to adverse effects on health and functioning. Self-regulation is the “act of managing cognition and emotion to enable goal-directed actions such as organizing behavior, controlling impulses, and solving problems constructively.”36 Three interrelated domains are involved in self-regulation: cognitive (eg, executive functioning and goal setting), emotional (eg, emotional awareness and management), and behavioral (eg, impulse control and delay of gratification). Self-regulation is governed by a number of brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and ventral striatum, which are undergoing profound developmental changes during adolescence37,38 and whose function is adversely affected by insufficient sleep.39–41

Short-term experimental sleep manipulation in young children,42,43 school-age children,44,45 and adolescents46 acutely affects self-regulation and the performance of complex tasks. Although impaired self-regulation has also been observed in the context of “naturalistic” chronic sleep loss, on a level experienced by many adolescents, there is some evidence that the relationship between sleep and functional impairments related to poor self-regulation involves more than insufficient sleep. For example, chronotype, or circadian-based morningness–eveningness preference (ie, the tendency to be a “morning lark” or “night owl”) is another aspect of sleep regulation that may independently influence self-regulation. A pronounced shift to a more evening chronotype coincides both with increased age and pubertal development in adolescents.47–50 Given that evening types cannot easily fall asleep earlier at night and must still meet societal demands to wake up and function early in the morning, this often results in a sleep duration that is insufficient to meet sleep needs. Moreover, timing of activity and alertness patterns during the day are also shifted later in these people. This misalignment of circadian biology and societal demands has been called “social jet lag.”51,52 Eveningness is also a risk factor in adolescents for a number of conditions that have also been associated with poor self-regulation: emotional and behavioral problems,53–56 substance abuse,57 obesity,58 health risk behaviors,52,59,60 and lower school performance.61–64 These findings suggest that a broader conceptualization of deficient sleep in adolescents that involves problematic timing and duration of sleep may be a useful construct in assessment of functional outcomes such as impaired self-regulation.

Finally, 2 cross-sectional studies of adolescents found that self-regulation was associated with daytime sleepiness (an increased propensity to fall asleep in inappropriate circumstances and difficulty maintaining alertness, which interferes with activities of daily living) but not nighttime sleep duration.65,66 However, neither study examined the contribution of chronotype to self-regulation.

The purpose of this study was to determine whether 3 distinct aspects of sleep, nighttime sleep duration, daytime sleepiness, and chronotype, were independently associated with self-regulation in a large community-based sample of middle and high school students. We secondarily examined the relationship between these dimensions of sleep and 3 domains of self-regulation: cognitive, emotional, and behavioral.

Methods

To determine how nighttime sleep duration, daytime sleepiness, and chronotype predict self-regulation, we used data from an online survey of students (grades 7–12) and parents, which was conducted between March and June 2015 in Fairfax County Public Schools. Fairfax is the 11th largest school district in the United States, serving a diverse community in the northern Virginia suburbs of Washington, District of Columbia.

Survey Design

The survey was made available to all students (and their parents) in 19 schools: 8 of 23 middle schools (grades 7–8), 8 of 24 high schools (grades 9–12), and all 3 secondary schools (grades 7–12). The 19 schools were selected to be representative of the entire district with regard to student race and ethnicity and family income. The survey design addressed the need to protect class time (no in-class administration), minimize disruptions (no direct e-mail or cell phone contact with students by the research team), and obtain parent consent. Parents (or primary caregivers) at each of the study schools received an initial invitation to participate in the study via a district-sponsored e-mail, followed by 3 reminders. Parents were asked to grant electronic consent for their child to participate and to complete an online parent survey. Students with parental consent were sent a link via e-mail to a separate online student survey. Upon completion of the survey, students were given a $5 gift card to Amazon.

We received 2020 complete student surveys from the ∼34 800 students enrolled in the 19 schools (6%). We excluded from our analysis 1 student age <12.0 years and 2 students >19.0 years, leaving a final sample of 2017 students. Compared with the overall population of students in grades 7 to 12 in the district, those in our sample were more often non-Hispanic white (60.7% vs 42.1%) and less often receiving free or reduced-price school meals (8.1% vs 27.4%). Study procedures were approved by the school district’s Research Screening Committee and the institutional review boards at the Children’s National Medical Center and Temple University.

Self-Regulation

Self-regulation, our study outcome, was measured with the validated Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition, Screening Self-Report Form, consisting of 12 statements about behaviors.67 Students were asked whether they “had problems with these behaviors over the last 6 months” and were given the response options of “never” (1), “sometimes” (2), or “often” (3). In addition to a total self-regulation score (Executive Function Screening score), 3 distinct subscores were generated: cognitive self-regulation, which has 6 items dealing with the facets of working memory (eg, “I forget instructions easily”), task completion (eg, “I have difficulty finishing a task on my own”), and planning or organizing (eg, “I don’t plan ahead for school assignments”); emotional self-regulation, which has 4 items dealing with the facets of shifts (eg, “It bothers me when I have to deal with changes”) and control (eg, “I get upset over small events”); and behavioral, which has 2 items dealing with the facet of inhibition (eg, “I am impulsive”).67 Item scores were reverse-coded so that higher scores reflected greater self-regulation. The total score and the 3 subscores, each with a possible range from 1 to 3, were calculated by adding across the constituent items and dividing by the number of items. In our sample, the internal consistency measures (Cronbach’s α) of the total score and the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral subscores were 0.85, 0.80, 0.71, and 0.65, respectively. The total self-regulation score and 3 subscores had statistically normal distributions.

Sleep Duration

Nighttime sleep duration on school nights was calculated from student responses to separate questions about usual bedtime on school nights and wake time on school days. Sleep duration was calculated as the difference in hours between these times after we excluded 27 cases with either implausible bedtimes (<7 pm or >3 am), wake times (<3 am or >8:30 am), or calculated duration (<4 or >12 hours). Sleep duration (hours) had a statistically normal distribution and was used in our primary analysis as a continuous variable.

Chronotype

Chronotype was assessed with the 10-item Morningness–Eveningness Scale for Children.50 The scale, adapted from circadian rhythm questionnaires for adults,68–70 is designed to assess the phase of the day, on a continuum from morningness to eveningness, in which adolescents prefer to engage in various cognitive and physical activities. The scale generates possible scores from 10 to 43, with higher scores indicating greater morningness. In our sample, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.79. The chronotype scores had a statistically normal distribution, and the score was used in our primary analysis as a continuous variable. There are no established cutoff points for defining morningness or eveningness on this scale.71 To facilitate the interpretation of our findings, additional analyses were conducted with the scores divided into tertiles: morningness (>28), intermediate (25–28), and eveningness (<25).

Daytime Sleepiness

Self-reported daytime sleepiness was assessed with a modified version of the 10-item Sleepiness Scale in the Sleep Habits Survey.72–74 Students were asked whether, during the last 2 weeks, they had “struggled to stay awake or fallen asleep” in 10 situations. We replaced 2 situations (“traveling in a bus, train, plane or car” and “attending a performance”) with 2 others (“during a study hall or free period” and “during lunch at school”). Our modified response options were “never” (0), “Yes, I struggled to stay awake” (1), and “Yes, I fell asleep” (2). Students could also indicate that the situation did not apply to them during the last 2 weeks. A total possible score between 0 and 2 was calculated by summing across the items (situations) and dividing the number of items and situations that applied to the student. Higher sleepiness scores meant greater daytime sleepiness. The Cronbach’s α of the modified scale was 0.77. Responses on the scale were skewed toward low sleepiness, with 28.1% scoring 0 (never sleepy during any of the situations). Therefore, scores were divided into 3 tertiles of sleepiness: low (0 to ≤0.1), medium (>0.1 to <0.4), and high (≥0.4).

Covariates

Data were obtained on potential confounding variables from the student survey, the parent survey, and electronic administrative records from the school district. Students reported their race and Hispanic ethnicity in separate questions. These data were merged into a 5-category race/ethnicity variable. Those reporting >1 race were placed in the “other” category, and those reporting Hispanic ethnicity, regardless of race designation, were classified as Hispanic. Students also reported whether they had any “disabilities or chronic illnesses,” including 2 mental health conditions: depression and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Students could also name other disabilities or chronic illnesses. Those naming anxiety disorders (eg, posttraumatic stress disorder) or other mood disorders (eg, bipolar disorder) were grouped, along with those reporting depression, into a single group called “anxiety and/or mood disorder.” Each parent respondent provided information on his or her own educational level and that of his or her spouse or partner. The school district provided data (yes/no) on student qualification for free or reduced-price school meals (household income ≤185% of the federal poverty guideline75), sex, and birth date (used to calculate age).

Data Analysis

Correlation coefficients were used to examine bivariate relationships between the 3 exposures (sleep duration, sleepiness, and chronotype). Each self-regulation measure was used as a dependent variable in multivariable linear regression analyses. Missing data were imputed for all study variables, and no systematic patterns of missing data were identified.76 In Stata (Stata Corp, College Station, TX),77 we used sequential regression imputation78 to create 20 imputed data sets, each with 2017 cases.79 Our imputation model included all 7 covariates, along with the self-regulation outcomes and key exposures (sleep duration, sleepiness, and chronotype). We then ran linear regression models on the imputed data sets, and the reported model parameters were aggregated across data sets.80 All 3 sleep variables were examined together in each model to determine their independent association with self-regulation, both with and without adjustment for all covariates. In models containing all covariates, we evaluated 2-way and 3-way interactions between the 3 sleep variables and between age and each sleep variable. Because multiple interactions were evaluated for each self-regulation outcome, a P value of <.01 was considered a statistically significant interaction.

Results

The mean age of the 2017 students was 15.0 (range 12.1–18.9) years, and 54.4% were in grades 9 to 12. Nearly 90% of students had at least 1 parent or primary caregiver who had completed college, and <10% qualified for free or reduced-price school meals (Table 1). In contrast to the 2016 American Academy of Sleep Medicine consensus recommendations for optimal sleep duration in 13-18 year olds (8-10 hours),81 21.7% reported a usual school-night sleep duration <7 hours.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1

Participant Characteristics

Correlations between sleep duration, sleepiness, and chronotype were all statistically significant (P < .001), reflecting greater daytime sleepiness among those with shorter sleep duration (Spearman r = −0.42) and those with greater eveningness preference (or lower chronotype score) (Spearman r = −0.44). Students with greater eveningness preference had shorter sleep duration (Pearson r = 0.34).

In linear regression models with self-regulation measures as the dependent variables and adjustment for all covariates, both sleepiness and chronotype were statistically significant (P ≤ .001) independent predictors of self-regulation (Table 2). However, in none of these models was sleep duration (in hours) significantly associated with self-regulation after we controlled for sleepiness and chronotype. This was also true when sleep duration was modeled as a binary (<7 and ≥7 hours) or 4-category (<7, 7 to <8, 8 to <9, and ≥9 hours) variable. The difference in total self-regulation score, expressed in SD units, between those with high and low sleepiness was 0.59 (95% confidence interval, 0.48–0.71), after we adjusted for covariates, chronotype score, and sleep duration (in hours). The difference in standardized total self-regulation score between those with eveningness and morningness chronotype was 0.35 (95% confidence interval, 0.24–0.46), after we adjusted for covariates, level of sleepiness, and sleep duration (in hours).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 2

Linear Regression Models Predicting Self-Regulation

When interaction terms were added to our regression models, none of the interactions were statistically significant. Specifically, the relationship between self-regulation and each of the sleep variables did not significantly differ by age. In groups with either shorter (<7 hours) or longer (≥7 hours) sleep duration, the relationship between self-regulation and sleepiness (Fig 1) was similar, as was the relationship between self-regulation and chronotype (Fig 2).

FIGURE 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
FIGURE 1

Adjusted mean self-regulation subscores by school-night sleep duration and daytime sleepiness. Group mean self-regulation subscores are adjusted for chronotype and 7 covariates (age, sex, race or ethnicity, highest parental education, receipt of free or reduced-price meals, anxiety or mood disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) and after multiple imputation of missing data (N = 2017).

FIGURE 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
FIGURE 2

Adjusted mean self-regulation subscores by school-night sleep duration and chronotype. Group mean self-regulation subscores are adjusted for sleepiness and 7 covariates (age, sex, race or ethnicity, highest parental education, receipt of free or reduced-price meals, anxiety or mood disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) and after multiple imputation of missing data (N = 2017).

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first study to examine sleep duration, sleepiness, and chronotype in relation to self-regulation in a large community-based sample of adolescents. Our findings suggest that eveningness chronotype and increased daytime sleepiness are associated with self-regulation but that short sleep duration is not.

These findings contrast with those from short-term experimental studies in adolescents showing that sleep restriction impairs self-regulation.82–87 However, those studies differ from larger epidemiologic studies, such as ours, which assessed the relationship between self-regulation and chronic sleep loss, based on usual nighttime sleep duration. Our findings are consistent with the only 2 other epidemiologic studies on this topic, both of which also showed no significant association among adolescents between sleep duration and aspects of self-regulation. Among 236 US adolescents, daytime sleepiness, but not nighttime sleep duration, predicted parent-reported deficits in executive function65; among 1194 Argentinian adolescents, sleep duration was not significantly associated with attention after accounting for daytime sleepiness.66 However, neither study examined the role of chronotype.

An emerging literature suggests that eveningness chronotype (circadian preference) is associated with behaviors involving impaired self-regulation, such as drug and alcohol use, and unsafe behaviors and sexual practices.26,88 Eveningness is also associated with risk taking in the domains of financial, ethical, and recreational decision-making.89 Furthermore, associations between chronotype, health risk behaviors, and well-being are strongest in adolescents and young adults.51

There are several possible explanations for this association. One is that evening types sleep less because of their later bedtimes and social and environmental demands necessitating early morning wake times. These people are also forced to awaken and function at a time of day that, for them, occurs during their biological lowest level of alertness (circadian nadir). Evening types are also more likely to be exposed to environmental factors (eg, increased late evening light and decreased morning light), which exacerbate the biologically based eveningness preference. In addition, throughout the day, circadian-based peaks and troughs of alertness in evening types may not coincide (or synchronize) with timing of environmental demands (eg, test taking and homework times).47,90,91 Thus, among evening-type adolescents the misalignment of environmental expectations with circadian biology may contribute to actual and perceived impairment in self-regulation. Furthermore, when given the opportunity on weekends and non–school or work days, evening types tend to revert to their normal sleep pattern of late bedtimes and wake times, which typically results in variability in sleep patterns over the course of an average week. All these factors are thought to contribute to the phenomenon of social jet lag or circadian misalignment.

Independent of chronotype, sleepiness was also associated with lower self-regulation. In adolescents, sleepiness may be a more accurate indicator than sleep duration of insufficient sleep because it reflects the person’s perception of sleep-related impairment. Sleepiness is probably influenced by factors other than sleep duration that have yet to be elucidated. One of those factors may be chronotype, which we found explained variability in self-regulation, independent of sleepiness or sleep duration. We also found that the negative impacts of increased sleepiness and eveningness chronotype on self-regulation were of similar magnitude regardless of sleep duration. Our findings suggest that clinicians may want to assess daytime sleepiness in adolescents by using existing tools92,93 and consider whether sleepiness, not just sleep duration, may be a factor in assessing the adequacy of adolescents’ sleep. Researchers should examine the etiology of sleepiness among adolescents, especially among those sleeping ≥8 hours each night, and the contribution of sleepiness to health and functioning.

Greater self-regulation in childhood is associated with a variety of positive adult outcomes, such as better physical health, more financial security, less criminality, and less substance use,94 and it is possible that improving early self-regulation may promote health and functioning over the life course. Our data suggest that sleep-related variables contribute to better self-regulation, and future research should prospectively evaluate the impact of multiple dimensions of sleep on the development of self-regulation. Although it is possible that poor self-regulation could contribute to unhealthy sleep practices that in turn result in sleep deficits, reverse causality would not be consistent with experimental studies in children showing that short-term manipulation of sleep alters self-regulation. Our results also have policy implications. For example, both genetic variations in clock genes and environmental influences contribute to the distribution of chronotypes in a given population.95 Thus, there may be remediable environmental factors that potentially contribute to chronic sleep loss, circadian misalignment, and increased daytime sleepiness in adolescents. In particular, early (before 8:30 am) school start times, which have been associated with chronic insufficient sleep, increased daytime sleepiness, and social jet lag, especially in eveningness students, may have long-term detrimental effects on adolescent health and functioning.13,96

There are limitations to this study. Because of the cross-sectional nature of the design, we cannot infer any causal relationships between variables, nor can we exclude the possibility that poor self-regulation impairs sleep. All our data were self-reported, which may lead to common rater bias. The results related to behavioral self-regulation should be interpreted cautiously because this subscore was derived from 2 items, which may also explain the low internal consistency of the measure. Our sample was not representative of the student population from which it was drawn, and this limitation may reduce the generalizability of the results. However, it is often unnecessary to have representative samples to make valid assessment of the associations between exposures and outcomes.97

Conclusions

Self-regulation in adolescents contributes to a range of positive health and functioning outcomes that have potential long-term implications. The development of self-regulation during adolescence may be adversely affected by exposure to sleep-related stressors, such as the circadian misalignment associated with evening chronotype and increased sleepiness, both of which may be related to the timing and duration of nighttime sleep. Efforts to optimize adolescents’ nighttime sleep, including implementation of healthy school start times and education on healthy sleep habits, may mitigate these effects.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to J. Michael Oakes, PhD, for his statistical consultation and Heather Polonsky for her clerical assistance.

Footnotes

    • Accepted September 1, 2016.
  • Address correspondence to Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH, Temple University, 3223 North Broad St, Suite 175, Philadelphia, PA 19140. E-mail: rwhitaker{at}temple.edu
  • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

  • FUNDING: Support for this study was provided in part by grants (72549, 73364, 73346) from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation.

  • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: Dr Gioia, as author and developer of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, 2nd edition, receives royalties for its use; the other authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

  • COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2016-3157.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Basch CE,
    2. Basch CH,
    3. Ruggles KV,
    4. Rajan S
    . Prevalence of sleep duration on an average school night among 4 nationally representative successive samples of American high school students, 2007–2013. Prev Chronic Dis. 2014;11:E216pmid:25496556
    OpenUrlPubMed
    1. Keyes KM,
    2. Maslowsky J,
    3. Hamilton A,
    4. Schulenberg J
    . The great sleep recession: changes in sleep duration among US adolescents, 1991–2012. Pediatrics. 2015;135(3):460–468pmid:25687142
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Knutson KL,
    2. Lauderdale DS
    . Sociodemographic and behavioral predictors of bed time and wake time among US adolescents aged 15 to 17 years. J Pediatr. 2009;154(3):426–430, 430.e1pmid:18849051
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Maslowsky J,
    2. Ozer EJ
    . Developmental trends in sleep duration in adolescence and young adulthood: evidence from a national United States sample. J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(6):691–697pmid:24361237
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Owens J; Adolescent Sleep Working Group; Committee on Adolescence
    . Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/134/3/e921pmid:25157012
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Urner M,
    2. Tornic J,
    3. Bloch KE
    . Sleep patterns in high school and university students: a longitudinal study. Chronobiol Int. 2009;26(6):1222–1234pmid:19731114
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Eaton DK,
    2. McKnight-Eily LR,
    3. Lowry R,
    4. Perry GS,
    5. Presley-Cantrell L,
    6. Croft JB
    . Prevalence of insufficient, borderline, and optimal hours of sleep among high school students: United States, 2007. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(4):399–401pmid:20307832
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Kim SJ,
    2. Lee YJ,
    3. Cho SJ,
    4. Cho IH,
    5. Lim W,
    6. Lim W
    . Relationship between weekend catch-up sleep and poor performance on attention tasks in Korean adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(9):806–812pmid:21893646
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. John B
    . Sleep-patterns, sleep hygiene behaviors and parental monitoring among Bahrain-based Indian adolescents. J Family Med Prim Care. 2015;4(2):232–237pmid:25949973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Morioka H,
    2. Itani O,
    3. Kaneita Y, et al
    . Associations between sleep disturbance and alcohol drinking: a large-scale epidemiological study of adolescents in Japan. Alcohol. 2013;47(8):619–628pmid:24188738
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Olds T,
    2. Maher C,
    3. Blunden S,
    4. Matricciani L
    . Normative data on the sleep habits of Australian children and adolescents. Sleep. 2010;33(10):1381–1388pmid:21061861
    OpenUrlPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Loessl B,
    2. Valerius G,
    3. Kopasz M,
    4. Hornyak M,
    5. Riemann D,
    6. Voderholzer U
    . Are adolescents chronically sleep-deprived? An investigation of sleep habits of adolescents in the southwest of Germany. Child Care Health Dev. 2008;34(5):549–556pmid:18549435
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Adolescent Sleep Working Group
    2. Committee on Adolescence
    3. Council on School Health
    . School start times for adolescents. Pediatrics. 2014;134(3):642–649pmid:25156998
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. American Medical Association
    . American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Resolution 503: Insufficient Sleep in Adolescents. Chicago, IL: American Medical Association, American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2010
  7. ↵
    1. US Department of Health and Human Services. Office of Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.
    Sleep health. Healthy People 2020 topics and objectives. Available at: www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topicsobjectives2020/objectiveslist.aspx?topicId=38. Accessed February 24, 2016
  8. ↵
    1. Fatima Y,
    2. Doi SAR,
    3. Mamun AA
    . Longitudinal impact of sleep on overweight and obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic review and bias-adjusted meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2015;16(2):137–149pmid:25589359
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Koren D,
    2. O’Sullivan KL,
    3. Mokhlesi B
    . Metabolic and glycemic sequelae of sleep disturbances in children and adults. Curr Diab Rep. 2015;15(1):562pmid:25398202
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Matthews KA,
    2. Pantesco EJ
    . Sleep characteristics and cardiovascular risk in children and adolescents: an enumerative review. Sleep Med. 2016;18:36–49pmid:26459685
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Ruan H,
    2. Xun P,
    3. Cai W,
    4. He K,
    5. Tang Q
    . Habitual sleep duration and risk of childhood obesity: systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Sci Rep. 2015;5:16160pmid:26537869
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Raniti MB,
    2. Allen NB,
    3. Schwartz O, et al
    . Sleep duration and sleep quality: associations with depressive symptoms across adolescence [published online ahead of print January 8, 2016]. Behav Sleep Med.doi:10.1080/15402002.2015.1120198pmid:26744783
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Wong MM,
    2. Brower KJ
    . The prospective relationship between sleep problems and suicidal behavior in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(7):953–959pmid:22551658
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Hysing M,
    2. Sivertsen B,
    3. Stormark KM,
    4. O’Connor RC
    . Sleep problems and self-harm in adolescence. Br J Psychiatry. 2015;207(4):306–312pmid:26206862
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Ireland JL,
    2. Culpin V
    . The relationship between sleeping problems and aggression, anger, and impulsivity in a population of juvenile and young offenders. J Adolesc Health. 2006;38(6):649–655pmid:16730591
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Haynes PL,
    2. Bootzin RR,
    3. Smith L,
    4. Cousins J,
    5. Cameron M,
    6. Stevens S
    . Sleep and aggression in substance-abusing adolescents: results from an integrative behavioral sleep-treatment pilot program. Sleep. 2006;29(4):512–520pmid:16676785
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Sivertsen B,
    2. Skogen JC,
    3. Jakobsen R,
    4. Hysing M
    . Sleep and use of alcohol and drug in adolescence. A large population-based study of Norwegian adolescents aged 16 to 19 years. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;149:180–186pmid:25707706
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. O’Brien EM,
    2. Mindell JA
    . Sleep and risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Behav Sleep Med. 2005;3(3):113–133pmid:15984914
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Gromov I,
    2. Gromov D
    . Sleep and substance use and abuse in adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2009;18(4):929–946pmid:19836697
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Danner F,
    2. Phillips B
    . Adolescent sleep, school start times, and teen motor vehicle crashes. J Clin Sleep Med. 2008;4(6):533–535pmid:19110880
    OpenUrlPubMed
    1. Vorona RD,
    2. Szklo-Coxe M,
    3. Wu A,
    4. Dubik M,
    5. Zhao Y,
    6. Ware JC
    . Dissimilar teen crash rates in two neighboring southeastern Virginia cities with different high school start times. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(2):145–151pmid:21509328
    OpenUrlPubMed
    1. Lucidi F,
    2. Russo PM,
    3. Mallia L,
    4. Devoto A,
    5. Lauriola M,
    6. Violani C
    . Sleep-related car crashes: risk perception and decision-making processes in young drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2006;38(2):302–309pmid:16288959
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Kim SY,
    2. Sim S,
    3. Kim SG,
    4. Choi HG
    . Sleep deprivation is associated with bicycle accidents and slip and fall injuries in Korean adolescents. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0135753pmid:26280345
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Graves JM,
    2. Miller ME
    . Reduced sleep duration and history of work-related injuries among Washington State adolescents with a history of working. Am J Ind Med. 2015;58(4):464–471pmid:25712405
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Silverberg ND,
    2. Berkner PD,
    3. Atkins JE,
    4. Zafonte R,
    5. Iverson GL
    . Relationship between short sleep duration and preseason concussion testing. Clin Sports Med. 2016;26(3):226–231
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    1. Dewald JF,
    2. Meijer AM,
    3. Oort FJ,
    4. Kerkhof GA,
    5. Bögels SM
    . The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(3):179–189pmid:20093054
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Hysing M,
    2. Harvey AG,
    3. Linton SJ,
    4. Askeland KG,
    5. Sivertsen B
    . Sleep and academic performance in later adolescence: results from a large population-based study. J Sleep Res. 2016;25(3):318–324pmid:26825591
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Murray DW,
    2. Rosanbalm K,
    3. Christopoulos C,
    4. Hamoudi A
    . Self-Regulation and Toxic Stress: Foundations for Understanding Self-Regulation From an Applied Developmental Perspective. Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services; 2014
  20. ↵
    1. Casey BJ
    . Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:295–319pmid:25089362
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Casey B,
    2. Caudle K
    . The teenage brain: self control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22(2):82–87pmid:25284961
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Hasler BP,
    2. Dahl RE,
    3. Holm SM, et al
    . Weekend–weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2012;91(3):334–341pmid:22960270
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Telzer EH,
    2. Fuligni AJ,
    3. Lieberman MD,
    4. Galván A
    . The effects of poor quality sleep on brain function and risk taking in adolescence. Neuroimage. 2013;71:275–283pmid:23376698
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Holm SM,
    2. Forbes EE,
    3. Ryan ND,
    4. Phillips ML,
    5. Tarr JA,
    6. Dahl RE
    . Reward-related brain function and sleep in pre/early pubertal and mid/late pubertal adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2009;45(4):326–334pmid:19766936
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Bernier A,
    2. Beauchamp MH,
    3. Bouvette-Turcot AA,
    4. Carlson SM,
    5. Carrier J
    . Sleep and cognition in preschool years: specific links to executive functioning. Child Dev. 2013;84(5):1542–1553pmid:23432661
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Molfese DL,
    2. Ivanenko A,
    3. Key AF, et al
    . A one-hour sleep restriction impacts brain processing in young children across tasks: evidence from event-related potentials. Dev Neuropsychol. 2013;38(5):317–336pmid:23862635
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Sadeh A,
    2. Gruber R,
    3. Raviv A
    . The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003;74(2):444–455pmid:12705565
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Gruber R,
    2. Cassoff J,
    3. Frenette S,
    4. Wiebe S,
    5. Carrier J
    . Impact of sleep extension and restriction on children’s emotional lability and impulsivity. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/130/5/e1155pmid:23071214
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Dewald-Kaufmann JF,
    2. Oort FJ,
    3. Meijer AM
    . The effects of sleep extension on sleep and cognitive performance in adolescents with chronic sleep reduction: an experimental study. Sleep Med. 2013;14(6):510–517pmid:23523432
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Adan A,
    2. Archer SN,
    3. Hidalgo MP,
    4. Di Milia L,
    5. Natale V,
    6. Randler C
    . Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29(9):1153–1175pmid:23004349
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Carskadon MA,
    2. Acebo C
    . Regulation of sleepiness in adolescents: update, insights, and speculation. Sleep. 2002;25(6):606–614pmid:12224839
    OpenUrlPubMed
    1. Carskadon MA,
    2. Acebo C,
    3. Jenni OG
    . Regulation of adolescent sleep: implications for behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:276–291pmid:15251897
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Carskadon MA,
    2. Vieira C,
    3. Acebo C
    . Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep. 1993;16(3):258–262pmid:8506460
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Wittmann M,
    2. Dinich J,
    3. Merrow M,
    4. Roenneberg T
    . Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23(1–2):497–509pmid:16687322
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Touitou Y
    . Adolescent sleep misalignment: a chronic jet lag and a matter of public health. J Physiol Paris. 2013;107(4):323–326pmid:23542542
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. de Souza CM,
    2. Hidalgo MPL
    . Midpoint of sleep on school days is associated with depression among adolescents. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(2):199–205pmid:24156519
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Díaz-Morales JF,
    2. Escribano C,
    3. Jankowski KS
    . Chronotype and time-of-day effects on mood during school day. Chronobiol Int. 2015;32(1):37–42pmid:25153134
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Schlarb AA,
    2. Sopp R,
    3. Ambiel D,
    4. Grünwald J
    . Chronotype-related differences in childhood and adolescent aggression and antisocial behavior: a review of the literature. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(1):1–16pmid:24147657
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Wang L,
    2. Chartrand TL
    . Morningness–eveningness and risk taking. J Psychol. 2015;149(3-4):394–411pmid:25901637
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hasler BP,
    2. Sitnick SL,
    3. Shaw DS,
    4. Forbes EE
    . An altered neural response to reward may contribute to alcohol problems among late adolescents with an evening chronotype. Psychiatry Res. 2013;214(3):357–364pmid:24144507
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Miller AL,
    2. Lumeng JC,
    3. LeBourgeois MK
    . Sleep patterns and obesity in childhood. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes. 2015;22(1):41–47pmid:25517022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Giannotti F,
    2. Cortesi F,
    3. Sebastiani T,
    4. Ottaviano S
    . Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence. J Sleep Res. 2002;11(3):191–199pmid:12220314
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Malone SK,
    2. Zemel B,
    3. Compher C, et al
    . Characteristics associated with sleep duration, chronotype, and social jet lag in adolescents. J Sch Nurs. 2016;32(2):120–131pmid:26376832
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Rahafar A,
    2. Maghsudloo M,
    3. Farhangnia S,
    4. Vollmer C,
    5. Randler C
    . The role of chronotype, gender, test anxiety, and conscientiousness in academic achievement of high school students. Chronobiol Int. 2016;33(1):1–9pmid:26651154
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Short MA,
    2. Gradisar M,
    3. Lack LC,
    4. Wright HR
    . The impact of sleep on adolescent depressed mood, alertness and academic performance. J Adolesc. 2013;36(6):1025–1033pmid:24215949
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Tonetti L,
    2. Fabbri M,
    3. Filardi M,
    4. Martoni M,
    5. Natale V
    . Effects of sleep timing, sleep quality and sleep duration on school achievement in adolescents. Sleep Med. 2015;16(8):936–940pmid:26116949
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Tonetti L,
    2. Natale V,
    3. Randler C
    . Association between circadian preference and academic achievement: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiol Int. 2015;32(6):792–801pmid:26125131
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Anderson B,
    2. Storfer-Isser A,
    3. Taylor HG,
    4. Rosen CL,
    5. Redline S
    . Associations of executive function with sleepiness and sleep duration in adolescents. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/123/4/e701pmid:19336360
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Perez-Lloret S,
    2. Videla AJ,
    3. Richaudeau A, et al
    . A multi-step pathway connecting short sleep duration to daytime somnolence, reduced attention, and poor academic performance: an exploratory cross-sectional study in teenagers. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9(5):469–473pmid:23674938
    OpenUrlPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Gioia GA,
    2. Isquith PK,
    3. Guy SC,
    4. Kenworthy L
    . Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. 2nd ed. Lutz, FL: PAR Inc; 2015
  44. ↵
    1. Torsvall L,
    2. Åkerstedt T
    . A diurnal type scale. Construction, consistency and validation in shift work. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1980;6(4):283–290pmid:7195066
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Smith CS,
    2. Reilly C,
    3. Midkiff K
    . Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J Appl Psychol. 1989;74(5):728–738pmid:2793773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Horne JA,
    2. Ostberg O
    . A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110pmid:1027738
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Tonetti L,
    2. Adan A,
    3. Di Milia L,
    4. Randler C,
    5. Natale V
    . Measures of circadian preference in childhood and adolescence: a review. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30(5):576–582pmid:25726892
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Carskadon MA,
    2. Acebo C
    . A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development. J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190–195pmid:8323929
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Wolfson AR,
    2. Carskadon MA
    . Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev. 1998;69(4):875–887pmid:9768476
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Wolfson AR,
    2. Carskadon MA,
    3. Acebo C, et al
    . Evidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescents. Sleep. 2003;26(2):213–216pmid:12683482
    OpenUrlPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Tribiano JJ; Food and Nutrition Service of the United States Department of Agriculture
    , eds. Child Nutrition Programs—Income Eligibility Guidelines. Washington, DC: Federal Register; 2015:17026–17027
  50. ↵
    1. Cummings P
    . Missing data and multiple imputation. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(7):656–661pmid:23699969
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. StataCorp
    . Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2013
  52. ↵
    1. Raghunathan TE,
    2. Lepkowski JM,
    3. Van Hoewyk J,
    4. Solenberger P
    . A multivariate technique for multiply imputing missing values using a sequence of regression models. Surv Methodol. 2001;27(1):85–95
    OpenUrl
  53. ↵
    1. Graham JW,
    2. Olchowski AE,
    3. Gilreath TD
    . How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prev Sci. 2007;8(3):206–213pmid:17549635
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Schafer JL,
    2. Graham JW
    . Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147–177pmid:12090408
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Paruthi S,
    2. Brooks LJ,
    3. D’Ambrosio C, et al
    . Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: a consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(6):785–786pmid:27250809
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Baum KT,
    2. Desai A,
    3. Field J,
    4. Miller LE,
    5. Rausch J,
    6. Beebe DW
    . Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(2):180–190pmid:24889207
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Beebe DW,
    2. Rose D,
    3. Amin R
    . Attention, learning, and arousal of experimentally sleep-restricted adolescents in a simulated classroom. J Adolesc Health. 2010;47(5):523–525pmid:20970088
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Beebe DW,
    2. Simon S,
    3. Summer S,
    4. Hemmer S,
    5. Strotman D,
    6. Dolan LM
    . Dietary intake following experimentally restricted sleep in adolescents. Sleep. 2013;36(6):827–834pmid:23729925
    OpenUrlPubMed
    1. Cohen-Zion M,
    2. Shabi A,
    3. Levy S,
    4. Glasner L,
    5. Wiener A
    . Effects of partial sleep deprivation on information processing speed in adolescence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(4):388–398pmid:26892867
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Garner AA,
    2. Miller MM,
    3. Field J,
    4. Noe O,
    5. Smith Z,
    6. Beebe DW
    . Impact of experimentally manipulated sleep on adolescent simulated driving. Sleep Med. 2015;16(6):796–799pmid:25953298
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Rossa KR,
    2. Smith SS,
    3. Allan AC,
    4. Sullivan KA
    . The effects of sleep restriction on executive inhibitory control and affect in young adults. J Adolesc Health. 2014;55(2):287–292pmid:24602612
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Pasch KE,
    2. Laska MN,
    3. Lytle LA,
    4. Moe SG
    . Adolescent sleep, risk behaviors, and depressive symptoms: are they linked? Am J Health Behav. 2010;34(2):237–248pmid:19814603
    OpenUrlPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Ponzi D,
    2. Wilson MC,
    3. Maestripieri D
    . Eveningness is associated with higher risk-taking, independent of sex and personality. Psychol Rep. 2014;115(3):932–947pmid:25457099
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Fabbri M,
    2. Mencarelli C,
    3. Adan A,
    4. Natale V
    . Time-of-day and circadian typology on memory retrieval. Biol Rhythm Res. 2013;44(1):125–142
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    1. Fabbri M,
    2. Natale V,
    3. Adan A
    . Effect of time of day on arithmetic fact retrieval in a number-matching task. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2008;127(2):485–490pmid:17919442
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Drake C,
    2. Nickel C,
    3. Burduvali E,
    4. Roth T,
    5. Jefferson C,
    6. Pietro B
    . The Pediatric Daytime Sleepiness Scale (PDSS): sleep habits and school outcomes in middle-school children. Sleep. 2003;26(4):455–458pmid:12841372
    OpenUrlPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Spilsbury JC,
    2. Drotar D,
    3. Rosen CL,
    4. Redline S
    . The Cleveland Adolescent Sleepiness Questionnaire: a new measure to assess excessive daytime sleepiness in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3(6):603–612pmid:17993042
    OpenUrlPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Moffitt TE,
    2. Arseneault L,
    3. Belsky D, et al
    . A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(7):2693–2698pmid:21262822
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Harbard E,
    2. Allen NB,
    3. Trinder J,
    4. Bei B
    . What’s keeping teenagers up? Prebedtime behaviors and actigraphy-assessed sleep over school and vacation. J Adolesc Health. 2016;58(4):426–432pmid:26874590
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Minges KE,
    2. Redeker NS
    . Delayed school start times and adolescent sleep: a systematic review of the experimental evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 2016;28:86–95pmid:26545246
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Rothman KJ,
    2. Gallacher JEJ,
    3. Hatch EE
    . Why representativeness should be avoided. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(4):1012–1014pmid:24062287
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  • Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
Next
Back to top

Advertising Disclaimer »

In this issue

Pediatrics
Vol. 147, Issue 4
1 Apr 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
View this article with LENS
Next
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Self-Regulation and Sleep Duration, Sleepiness, and Chronotype in Adolescents
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Request Permissions
Article Alerts
Log in
You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Self-Regulation and Sleep Duration, Sleepiness, and Chronotype in Adolescents
Judith A. Owens, Tracy Dearth-Wesley, Daniel Lewin, Gerard Gioia, Robert C. Whitaker
Pediatrics Nov 2016, e20161406; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1406

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Self-Regulation and Sleep Duration, Sleepiness, and Chronotype in Adolescents
Judith A. Owens, Tracy Dearth-Wesley, Daniel Lewin, Gerard Gioia, Robert C. Whitaker
Pediatrics Nov 2016, e20161406; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1406
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Print
Download PDF
Insight Alerts

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Clinical Impact of a Diagnostic Gastrointestinal Panel in Children
  • Intrapartum Group B Streptococcal Prophylaxis and Childhood Allergic Disorders
  • Changes in Neurodevelopmental Outcomes From Age 2 to 10 Years for Children Born Extremely Preterm
Show more Article

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Sleep Medicine
    • Sleep Medicine
  • Adolescent Health/Medicine
    • Adolescent Health/Medicine
  • Journal Info
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Policies
  • Overview
  • Licensing Information
  • Authors/Reviewers
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submit My Manuscript
  • Open Access
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Librarians
  • Institutional Subscriptions
  • Usage Stats
  • Support
  • Contact Us
  • Subscribe
  • Resources
  • Media Kit
  • About
  • International Access
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • FAQ
  • AAP.org
  • shopAAP
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
  • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
  • RSS
American Academy of Pediatrics

© 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics