Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Review Article

Psychosocial Effects of Parent-Child Book Reading Interventions: A Meta-analysis

Qian-Wen Xie, Celia H.Y. Chan, Qingying Ji and Cecilia L.W. Chan
Pediatrics April 2018, 141 (4) e20172675; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-2675
Qian-Wen Xie
aDepartment of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Celia H.Y. Chan
aDepartment of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Qingying Ji
bShanghai Children’s Medical Center, Shanghai, China
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cecilia L.W. Chan
aDepartment of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

CONTEXT: Parent-child book reading (PCBR) is effective at improving young children’s language, literacy, brain, and cognitive development. The psychosocial effects of PCBR interventions are unclear.

OBJECTIVE: To systematically review and synthesize the effects of PCBR interventions on psychosocial functioning of children and parents.

DATA SOURCES: We searched ERIC, PsycINFO, Medline, Embase, PubMed, Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts, Social Services Abstracts, Sociological Abstracts, Family and Society Studies Worldwide, and Social Work Abstracts. We hand searched references of previous literature reviews.

STUDY SELECTION: Randomized controlled trials.

DATA EXTRACTION: By using a standardized coding scheme, data were extracted regarding sample, intervention, and study characteristics.

RESULTS: We included 19 interventions (3264 families). PCBR interventions improved the psychosocial functioning of children and parents compared with controls (standardized mean difference: 0.185; 95% confidence interval: 0.077 to 0.293). The assumption of homogeneity was rejected (Q = 40.010; P < .01). Two moderator variables contributed to between-group variance: method of data collection (observation less than interview; Qb = 7.497; P < .01) and rater (reported by others less than self-reported; Qb = 21.368; P < .01). There was no significant difference between effects of PCBR interventions on psychosocial outcomes of parents or children (Qb = 0.376; P = .540).

LIMITATIONS: The ratio of moderating variables to the included studies limited interpretation of the findings.

CONCLUSIONS: PCBR interventions are positively and significantly beneficial to the psychosocial functioning of both children and parents.

  • Abbreviations:
    CI —
    confidence interval
    CMA —
    Comprehensive Meta-Analysis
    CONSORT —
    Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials
    DR —
    dialogic reading
    PCBR —
    parent-child book reading
    RCT —
    randomized controlled trial
    SES —
    socioeconomic status
  • There is extensive literature in which researchers support the positive contributions of parent-child book reading (PCBR) experiences to early child development, especially language and literacy development.1,2 PCBR during early childhood is also a strong predictor of children’s brain development3 and later academic achievement.4 Given the benefits of PCBR, a Policy Statement from the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends that it is a responsibility of pediatric health care providers to encourage parents to read with their children as early as possible.5

    A large number of PCBR intervention programs have been implemented worldwide, such as the Reach Out and Read program in the United States and the Home Interaction Program for Parents and Youngsters in Australia. PCBR interventions have been suggested as an important tool in closing the achievement gap between families of high and middle socioeconomic status (SES) and families of lower SES.6 However, the lack of systematic evaluation of the effects of PCBR interventions has been a major criticism.7

    In the past, research on the effects of PCBR has been limited to the field of early childhood education. Recently, an increasing number of scholars have suggested looking at the benefits of PCBR in a broader way, rather than exclusively focusing on literacy or language development of children.8,9 To date, there have been few studies in which researchers have studied the psychosocial effects of PCBR on children, and their findings are inconsistent.10–14 Moreover, the role of parents as beneficiaries in PCBR interactions has been often ignored, despite some evidence that PCBR interventions could not only improve parenting competence15,16 and parent self-esteem17 but also reduce parent stress and depression.18,19

    Although PCBR has been recognized as an interactive activity between parents and children, limited research has been focused on its impact on the quality of parent-child relationships. In the 1990s, Bus et al20 demonstrated an association between PCBR activities and child-parent attachment relationships, whereas more recent studies have revealed mixed results.13,21

    Researchers have explored the predictors that might moderate the effectiveness of PCBR interventions in improving the language or literacy development of children. There are inconsistent findings from this research, with some evidence for children’s age,22,23 sex,24 race and/or ethnicity,1 and at-risk status25 and parents’ sex,26 educational background,27 and SES as predictors28; however, authors of other studies have suggested that PCBR was equally effective despite children’s age, ethnicity, at-risk status,29 and family’s SES.16 There is also the school of thought that different characteristics of PCBR interventions might produce different effects.2 For example, researchers suggest that children could benefit more from interventions by using dialogic reading (DR) techniques that emphasize high levels of adult-child interaction than traditional book reading.25,30

    PCBR interventions emphasize parent-child interactions and family empowerment rather than directly targeting developmental problems. It is therefore important to understand how well PCBR interventions work in enhancing psychosocial outcomes related to parent-child interactions. Psychosocial functioning encompasses various aspects of psychiatric, psychological, and social competence and well-being, and it refers to the ability of self-caring or working, a positive evaluation of self and life, and a positive well-being received from meaningful relationships or activities.31,32 Psychosocial functioning has usually been measured by symptom severity31 (eg, depression,33 stress symptoms,34 behavioral problems,35 etc), personal competence or skills31 (eg, personal performance, social-emotional adjustment,36 parental practices37), and sociocultural expectancies31 (eg, quality of life36 and parent-child relationships38).

    There is no synthesis of the available research on the impact of PCBR interventions on the psychosocial functioning of children and parents. Two research questions underpinned the meta-analysis reported in this article: (1) do PCBR interventions positively affect the psychosocial functioning of both children and parents, and (2) to what extent are these intervention effects moderated by sample characteristics, study characteristics, and intervention characteristics?

    Methods

    The meta-analysis was reported on the basis of the PRISMA reporting standard.39

    Eligibility Criteria

    Studies were included in this meta-analysis if the following were included:

    1. a PCBR intervention group that received structured training, supportive materials, or other reading-related services for encouraging parents to read books with their children was compared with a control group that did not;

    2. a randomized controlled trial (RCT) design was used;

    3. outcome variables were contained that were measures of psychosocial functioning of children or parents;

    4. sufficient empirical information to calculate effect sizes was provided; and

    5. the study was reported in the English language and published in peer-reviewed journals.

    Information Sources and Search Strategy

    Studies were identified by a comprehensive literature search through 10 electronic databases, including ERIC, PsycINFO, Medline, Embase, PubMed, Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts, Social Services Abstracts, Sociological Abstracts, Family and Society Studies Worldwide, and Social Work Abstracts. Search dates were from the date of inception to June 2017. Search terms comprised the following synonyms: (reading or literacy) and (parent-child or family or home) and (random* or experiment* or RCT). In addition, the reference lists of previous reviews1,2,25,29,40–43 were hand searched for relevant intervention studies.

    Study Selection and Quality Assessment

    All records were exported to EndNote software for the management of studies and elimination of duplicates.44 Titles, abstracts, and full texts of the remaining studies were scanned, according to the selection criteria. To assess the quality of each study, 2 investigators independently calculated the methodology quality score on the basis of the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) 2010 checklist.45 The checklist contained 10 items regarding the research method, such as trial design, participants, interventions, outcomes, sample size, randomization, blinding, and statistic methods. The score was coded as 1 for 1 item. A study received the highest score (10) when it satisfied all criteria.

    Data Collection

    By using a standardized coding scheme (Table 1), data items were extracted regarding sample characteristics, intervention characteristics, or study characteristics. Because multiple outcome measures were reported in this literature review, 2 study characteristics (ie, rater and method of data collection) were coded at the outcome domain level instead of the study level. We were interested in the effects of independent PCBR interventions. When researchers in a study compared 2 independent PCBR intervention groups to 1 control group, we treated the 2 independent PCBR interventions as 2 separate studies. Also, we equally divided the sample of the control group of the original study into 2 groups to prevent participants from being counted more than once. Similar procedures were used in previous meta-analysis studies.25,46

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 1

    Coding Scheme

    Two raters independently coded a random sample of 20% of the included studies to estimate the interrater reliability of the study codes. We calculated Cohen’s κ by using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY), in which a high level of agreement between the raters was revealed (unweighted κ = 0.88). After all coding inconsistencies between coders were resolved by discussion, 1 rater coded the remaining 80% of the studies.

    Synthesis of Results

    Calculations for the meta-analysis were performed by using the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) software.47 For each intervention, we computed an effect size as a standardized mean difference between the mean of a PCBR intervention group and a control group at posttest by using Cohen’s d.47 When an intervention contained more than 1 outcome domain, we treated each outcome domain as an independent correlate for comparing the effect sizes of different outcome domains. We averaged the effect sizes within the study if 1 outcome domain was measured by multiple tests.47 To avoid including more than 1 effect size per construct per sample, we aggregated the effect sizes of different outcome domains by means of averaging to generate a combined effect size47,48 called “total psychosocial functioning.” We converted each study-level treatment effect to a standardized mean difference for calculating an overall effect size of all included PCBR interventions on the psychosocial functioning of both children and parents. The precision of effect sizes was addressed by the 95% confidence interval (CI). A combined effect is considered significant if the CI does not include 0. The Q statistic was used to test the homogeneity across studies, and significant Qs imply heterogeneity. I2 was used to measure the degree of inconsistency between studies.

    Studies were grouped by relevant characteristics to test the impact of moderator variables. This analysis used the coding developed for studies, samples, and interventions (Table 1). These codes were applied as moderator variables for analysis of whether these characteristics were related to the effects of PCBR interventions on psychosocial outcomes of children and parents. The analyses of the impact of the data collection method and rater were conducted at outcome domain level. Other moderator variables were analyzed at study level.

    Publication Bias

    Omitting unpublished studies from this meta-analysis could bias the estimates of the effect of PCBR interventions because studies with significant findings might have more opportunities to be published in peer-reviewed journals than studies with nonsignificant findings. The CMA software was also used to test publication bias.47 Visual inspection of the funnel plot was used to address the potential impact of publication bias.49 The Begg and Mazumdar’s50 rank correlation test and Egger’s linear regression method51 were used to quantify the bias captured by the funnel plot. The Rosenthal’s fail-safe number was calculated, which reflects the number of missing studies with null or nonsignificant results that would have to be included in the meta-analysis before the P value becomes nonsignificant.47 We used the trim and fill approach to calculate the unbiased effect size if there appeared to be asymmetry around the point estimate.52

    Subgroup Analyses

    We treated outcomes from parents and children separately, as 2 subgroups, for comparing the effect sizes of psychosocial functioning for different groups of recipients (ie, children versus parents). In each study, we aggregated effects within a given intervention to generate a single effect size called “child psychosocial functioning” or “parent psychosocial functioning.”

    Results

    Study Inclusion

    The electronic database search yielded 3271 records. Hand searching reference lists of the earlier published reviews yielded 16 additional studies related to PCBR. After removing duplicates, 1974 studies remained. After further consideration of title, abstract, and full text, a total of 18 studies12–14,16,18,19,53–64 met the selection criteria and were included in the meta-analysis. In Fig 1, we present the study inclusion process.

    FIGURE 1
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 1

    Screening process of resources.

    In 1 of the 18 studies, 2 independent intervention groups were separately compared to 1 control group.14 We were interested in both interventions: one was a book gifting intervention, which provided free books to parents to encourage parents to read with their child; whereas the other included parental training related to PCBR. Therefore, we coded the 2 interventions as 2 separate studies. We labeled 1 intervention-control pair as Study 1 and the other as Study 2. Thus, 19 independent interventions reported in 18 relevant studies were assessed in the present meta-analysis.

    Outcome Measures

    Outcome measures of the psychosocial functioning of children included the following:

    1. social-emotional adjustment, assessed with the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment65 and the Parent Rating Scales from the Behavior Assessment System for Children, Second Edition66 and the Social Competence Scale67;

    2. behavior problems, assessed with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire,68 the Parental Account of Child Symptoms questionnaire,69 and the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory70;

    3. quality of life, assessed with the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory71; and

    4. reading interest, assessed with the Brief Reading Interest Scale60 and a self-designed questionnaire.

    Psychosocial functioning outcome measures for parents included the following:

    1. stress and/or depression, assessed with the Parenting Stress Index72,73 and the Beck Depression Inventory–Revised74;

    2. parenting competence, assessed with the Family Involvement Questionnaire,75 StimQ-P,76 Parent Involvement Questionnaire,77 and a self-designed questionnaire;

    3. parent-child relationship, assessed with a self-designed questionnaire; and

    4. parental attitude to reading with child, assessed with the Parent Reading Belief Inventory,78 and a self-designed questionnaire.

    Study Characteristics

    The characteristics of studies, participants, and interventions are presented in Table 2.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 2

    Characteristics of Participants and Interventions

    Characteristics of Studies

    Of the 18 studies, only 3 were published earlier than 2010.56,58,60 Sample sizes across 19 interventions ranged from 15 to 584 individuals. Although all interventions included were evaluated by using RCTs, the scores of their methodological quality ranged from 4 to 9.

    Characteristics of Participants

    Nineteen interventions (N subjects = 3264) were provided to different types of children and families. Ten interventions14,18,19,53,56–60 targeted infants and/or toddlers (0–3 years old; n = 1856) and 9 interventions12,13,16,54,55,61–64 were tested with preschool-aged children (3–6 years old; n = 1408). The percent of girls participating ranged from 35% to 78%. Of the 15 studies in which researchers reported sufficient information of participants, ∼44% were members of ethnic minorities. The majority of the interventions were delivered to children living in at-risk situations (eg, having low incomes, less-educated mothers, behavioral problems, language delay, or living in disadvantaged communities).* Mothers were the most common parent included in the interventions. Parents were reported as having low education in 11 interventions (n = 2024).12–14,18,53,54,61–64 There were 13 interventions provided to families of low SES (n = 2436).12–14,18,19,53–57,61,62 Ten interventions12,14,18,53–56,58,60 were conducted in the United States (n = 1495), and the other 913,16,19,57,59,61–64 were also from high-income countries or areas such as the United Kingdom, Australia, and Hong Kong (n = 1769).

    Characteristics of Interventions

    Interventions were conducted in a range of contexts, including primary care centers or hospitals, participants’ homes, schools, and communities (eg, library, Head Start center, or laboratory). DR techniques were used in 5 interventions (n = 1227).12,16,54,57,58 Five interventions combined PCBR activities with other psychosocial components, such as parenting or child behavior programs (n = 955).13,14 (study 1),18,61,62 The majority of interventions provided parents with structured training on how to read with children (n = 2704). Dosage of training ranged from 2 to 28 sessions. Services were delivered to families by using individual models (n = 2593) or group models (n = 671). Nine interventions also delivered home visit services to families (n = 1475).12,13,53,54,61–64 Only 3 interventions employed professionals (eg, people with university degrees in early education or speech pathology) to deliver services (n = 595).12,16,54 The duration of studies (from pretest to posttest) varied considerably. The longest study lasted for 48 months whereas the shortest one lasted 1 month.

    Synthesis of Results

    The random effect sizes pooled by all the outcomes for each study are presented in Fig 2. There was considerable variability in the effect sizes reported in the included studies. Seventeen interventions affected children’s and parents’ psychosocial functioning positively, and negative impacts were demonstrated in 2 interventions. Combining results from 19 studies yielded a weighted mean effect on general psychosocial functioning of children and parents of d = 0.185 (95% CI: 0.077 to 0.293), which is a small effect size79 of the total psychosocial functioning. The results of z tests (z = 3.355; P = .001) revealed that the overall effect size differed significantly from 0. Thus, the interventions included in this meta-analysis had a small but statistically significant effect on the psychosocial outcomes of children and parents. The statistically significant Q statistic of 40.010 (P = .002) indicated that the differences among the effect sizes were due to heterogeneity rather than participant-level sampling error.39 The I2 value (I2 = 55.011) indicated that ∼55% of total variance among studies was due to heterogeneity.

    FIGURE 2
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 2

    Random effect size for each study. df, degrees of freedom.

    Publication Bias

    The CMA software created a funnel plot (Fig 3) of any effect size index on the x-axis by the SE on the y-axis, which visually assessed the possibility of publication bias. Most studies were distributed symmetrically around the combined effect size. Studies at the bottom are clustered toward the right-hand side of the graph, making the effect size bigger than the unbiased effect size. For the rank correlation test, Kendall’s τ is 0.345 (P = .019). For Egger’s test, the intercept (b) is 1.289, with a 95% CI from −0.522 to 3.099 (P = .076). The test of Egger’s regression and Begg and Mazumdar’s50 rank correlation revealed obscure asymmetry in the funnel plot. The classic fail-safe number indicated that 119 additional studies with null or nonsignificant results needed to be added to overturn these significant results negatively. Under the random effects model, the unbiased effect size was 0.174, slightly smaller than 0.185, which indicates that there is a tiny gap between the real effectiveness and the calculated effectiveness.

    FIGURE 3
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 3

    Publication bias.

    Explaining the Variability in Effect Sizes

    Because the assumption of homogeneity between studies was rejected, further analysis was undertaken to assess whether the characteristics of the studies could account for the variance. Among the 21 variables listed in Table 1 that were analyzed for their impact as moderator variables, only 2 contributed significantly to between-group variance. These were the method of data collection (observation less than interview; Qb = 7.497; P < .001) and rater (reported by others less than self-reported; Qb = 21.368; P < .001).

    Subgroup Analyses

    In 14 interventions, researchers assessed the psychosocial performances of parents (n = 2642), and in 10 studies, researchers assessed the outcomes of children (n = 1884). Small effect sizes were found for parent psychosocial functioning (d = 0.219; 95% CI: 0.091 to 0.348; Q = 24.673; P = .025) and child psychosocial functioning (d = 0.157; 95% CI: 0.004 to 0.310; Q = 16.341; P = .060). Although the effect size of parents’ outcomes was larger than children’s, there was no significant difference in the effects of PCBR interventions on psychosocial outcomes of parents and children (Qb = 0.376; P = .540). Different psychosocial outcomes of parents and children were examined and findings are presented in Table 3.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 3

    Outcomes of the Effects of PCBR Interventions

    Discussion

    Summary of Evidence

    PCBR is commonly considered as one of the most important activities within the family context.28 Our meta-analysis was undertaken in an attempt to assess the effects of PCBR interventions on psychosocial functioning in general. Combining the results of 19 interventions and representing 3264 families, our analysis produced a mean weighted effect size that was small but significant (at 0.185). In our review, we suggest that PCBR interventions may be superior to control for improving the psychosocial functioning of both children and parents.

    PCBR is a complex social process occurring within an interpersonal context, which supports a broad range of outcomes for both children and their parents.26,27,80,81 Demonstrated in our synthesis is that PCBR interventions might positively impact children’s social-emotional competence, quality of life, and reading interest. Behaviors and responses of children may also impact the competence or well-being of parents. PCBR interventions might be effective in improving parents’ parenting competence, attitudes to reading with their child, and the quality of their relationships with children. It may also assist in reducing their stress or depression. We found no statistically significant difference in the impacts of PCBR interventions on psychosocial outcomes of parents and children. Thus, prioritizing 1 group of participants’ outcomes over another (whether children or parents) may ignore the potential of PCBR interactions.8

    In our review, we found that the psychosocial effects of PCBR were similar, despite the characteristics of participating children. Although age may impact the effectiveness of PCBR interventions in improving children’s acquisition of literacy,40 PCBR interventions appear to have similar psychosocial effects on both older children (3–6 years old) and younger children (0–3 years old). In contrast with previous research in which a child’s sex was considered as an important factor when interpreting PCBR interactions,24 our review did not find that a child’s sex could predict the psychosocial effects of PCBR interventions.

    In our review, we support that children who were socioeconomically or culturally disadvantaged might equally benefit from PCBR interventions as their counterparts. For example, we found that the psychosocial effects of PCBR were not dependent on the race and/or ethnicity of children, although it has been indicated in previous research that white families might benefit more from PCBR activities than minority families.1 Moreover, in this review, we did not find support for the expected moderating effects of at-risk status of children (eg, having low incomes, less-educated mothers, behavioral problems, language delay, or living in a disadvantaged community) on the psychosocial effects of PCBR interventions. In fact, children living in at-risk status or from families of low SES or a minority ethnicity may need more reading-related support because they may have fewer educational resources than their counterparts. In many previous studies, researchers have also shown the success of PCBR programs for children from high-risk families, such as children whose parents were in prison,82 children whose mothers were teenagers,19,83 and children who were from homeless families.84

    In the current review, we found that the length of the study and dosage of PCBR intervention were not predictive of psychosocial effectiveness. There are other variables that were not assessed in the included studies that may be influencing factors, such as the contents and text features of books22,24 and the quality of parent-child interactions.20,24 Moreover, provision of DR training may not have an impact on the general psychosocial effectiveness of PCBR interventions. It is suggested in the meta-analysis that shared reading as a meaningful interaction between children and parents rather than specific reading techniques might be the key to the positive psychosocial effects of PCBR interventions. We believe that PCBR is a low cost and simply adapted approach for any parent-child dyad, no matter what the circumstances.

    Conducting this meta-analysis allowed us to assess the current state of the research on PCBR interventions. We found a limited number of studies that met the selection criteria. Future researchers should pay more attention to the quality of study design because many PCBR-related studies identified in our search were excluded because of not employing an RCT design. Also, validated scales were not commonly used to evaluate psychosocial effects of PCBR interventions, especially on the quality of parent-child relationships. PCBR is not only a process of communicating information or learning skills but also a socially created, interactive process. Using validated scales to assess its effects on parent-child relationships may improve our understanding about the dynamics of PCBR interactions. In our review, we also identified that only a limited number of the reported PCBR interventions involved fathers. Future PCBR interventions should be designed to attract the participation of fathers because of the importance of father-child interactions in the development of children.85

    Limitations

    First, because of our strict inclusion criteria, we were able to include only a limited number of studies. The ratio of moderating variables to the included studies limits interpretation of the findings and potentially renders this review as an exploratory process. Second, dissecting interventions in the included studies was problematic because authors of some studies reported on interventions with combined reading and psychosocial components (eg, parenting programs and child behavior programs).13,61,62 It was therefore difficult to extract and conclude the role of PCBR components in these multiple interventions. Third, we identified and included a broad range of psychosocial outcomes from the included studies. For example, we included studies in which the effects of PCBR on reading interests of children and parental attitudes of reading with their child were assessed. To make sense of these different measures, we treated reading interest as personal competence of children and positive attitudes in reading with children as an important parenting competence. Whether the studies were similar enough to be combined may be questioned because of the various measures of psychosocial functioning included in the review. However, the goal of this review was to explore the pattern of psychosocial effectiveness of PCBR interventions by assessing psychosocial effectiveness of PCBR interventions in general. The method we used to calculate effect sizes was suggested by Borenstein et al47 and has been also used in previous studies as well.43,86 We recognized the limitation and addressed the diversity by applying the random effects model and reporting the range of true effects.47 We also interpreted the variability by testing the effects of moderator variables.

    Conclusions

    Exploring and assessing psychosocial effects of shared reading between parents and children allow us to extend the implications of PCBR interventions. A large number of family interventions have traditionally targeted behavioral problems of children instead of the interactions or relationships between parents and children. Because of the limited long-term efficacy of an individual-focused approach, more and more scholars have highlighted the importance of relationship-focused interventions.87,88 Moreover, psychosocial interventions targeting children have usually required traditional face-to-face therapies, which require intensive resources, especially including professional therapists. The delivery of these interventions has also posed challenges when children return to their families, if their families are not able to assist in the therapy.89 PCBR programs can improve psychosocial functioning of children through empowering parents, which may be more cost-effective than face-to-face therapies for children alone.

    In summary, suggested in our meta-analysis findings is that PCBR interventions might positively impact the psychosocial functioning of both parents and children. It seems prudent to consider the application of PCBR in improving the psychosocial well-being of families, especially those at high risk.

    Footnotes

      • Accepted January 25, 2018.
    • Address correspondence to Cecilia L.W. Chan, PhD, Room 545, Jockey Club Tower, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong. E-mail: cecichan{at}hku.hk
    • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

    • FUNDING: No external funding.

    • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

    • ↵* Refs 12–14,16,18,19,53–57,61–64.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. Manz PH,
      2. Hughes C,
      3. Barnabas E,
      4. Bracaliello C,
      5. Ginsburg-Block M
      . A descriptive review and meta-analysis of family-based emergent literacy interventions: to what extent is the research applicable to low-income, ethnic-minority or linguistically-diverse young children? Early Child Res Q. 2010;25(4):409–431
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    2. ↵
      1. Sénéchal M,
      2. Young L
      . The effect of family literacy interventions on children’s acquisition of reading from kindergarten to grade 3: a meta-analytic review. Rev Educ Res. 2008;78(4):880–907
      OpenUrl
    3. ↵
      1. Mustard J
      . Experience-based brain development: scientific underpinnings of the importance of early child development in a global world. Paediatr Child Health. 2006;11(9):571–572pmid:19030325
      OpenUrlPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. McElvany N,
      2. Artelt C
      . Systematic reading training in the family: development, implementation, and initial evaluation of the Berlin parent-child reading program. Learn Instr. 2009;19(1):79–95
      OpenUrl
    5. ↵
      1. Council on Early Childhood,
      2. High PC,
      3. Klass P
      . Literacy promotion: an essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014;134(2):404–409pmid:24962987
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    6. ↵
      1. Aram D,
      2. Fine Y,
      3. Ziv M
      . Enhancing parent-child shared book reading interactions: promoting references to the book’s plot and socio-cognitive themes. Early Child Res Q. 2013;28(1):111–122
      OpenUrl
    7. ↵
      1. Anderson J,
      2. Anderson A,
      3. Friedrich N,
      4. Kim JE
      . Taking stock of family literacy: some contemporary perspectives. J Early Child Literacy. 2010;10(1):33–53
      OpenUrl
    8. ↵
      1. St. Clair R
      . Reading, writing, and relationships: human and social capital in family literacy programs. Adult Basic Educ Lit J. 2008;2(2):84–93
      OpenUrl
    9. ↵
      1. Terry M
      . Exploring the Additive Benefit of Parental Nurturance Training on Parent and Child Shared Reading Outcomes: A Pilot Intervention Study [dissertation]. College Station, TX: Texas A&M University; 2012
    10. ↵
      1. Aram D,
      2. Aviram S
      . Mothers’ storybook reading and kindergartners’ socioemotional and literacy development. Read Psychol. 2009;30(2):175–194
      OpenUrl
      1. Curenton SM,
      2. Craig MJ
      . Shared-reading versus oral storytelling: associations with preschoolers’ prosocial skills and problem behaviours. Early Child Dev Care. 2011;181(1):123–146
      OpenUrl
    11. ↵
      1. Mathis ET,
      2. Bierman KL
      . Effects of parent and child pre-intervention characteristics on child skill acquisition during a school readiness intervention. Early Child Res Q. 2015;33:87–97pmid:27279678
      OpenUrlPubMed
    12. ↵
      1. O’Connor TG,
      2. Matias C,
      3. Futh A,
      4. Tantam G,
      5. Scott S
      . Social learning theory parenting intervention promotes attachment-based caregiving in young children: randomized clinical trial. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013;42(3):358–370pmid:23020146
      OpenUrlPubMed
    13. ↵
      1. Weisleder A,
      2. Cates CB,
      3. Dreyer BP, et al
      . Promotion of positive parenting and prevention of socioemotional disparities. Pediatrics. 2016;137(2):e20153239pmid:26817934
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    14. ↵
      1. Goldfeld S,
      2. Napiza N,
      3. Quach J,
      4. Reilly S,
      5. Ukoumunne OC,
      6. Wake M
      . Outcomes of a universal shared reading intervention by 2 years of age: the Let’s Read trial. Pediatrics. 2011;127(3):445–453pmid:21321030
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    15. ↵
      1. Lam S-F,
      2. Chow-Yeung K,
      3. Wong BPH,
      4. Lau KK,
      5. Tse SI
      . Involving parents in paired reading with preschoolers: results from a randomized controlled trial. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2013;38(2):126–135
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    16. ↵
      1. Swain J,
      2. Brooks G,
      3. Bosley S
      . The benefits of family literacy provision for parents in England. J Early Child Res. 2014;12(1):77–91
      OpenUrl
    17. ↵
      1. Cates CB,
      2. Weisleder A,
      3. Dreyer BP, et al
      . Leveraging healthcare to promote responsive parenting: impacts of the video interaction project on parenting stress. J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25(3):827–835pmid:27134514
      OpenUrlPubMed
    18. ↵
      1. Kumar MM,
      2. Cowan HR,
      3. Erdman L,
      4. Kaufman M,
      5. Hick KM
      . Reach out and read is feasible and effective for adolescent mothers: a pilot study. Matern Child Health J. 2016;20(3):630–638pmid:26520158
      OpenUrlPubMed
    19. ↵
      1. Bus AG,
      2. Belsky J,
      3. van IJzendoorn MH,
      4. Crnic K
      . Attachment and bookreading patterns: a study of mothers, fathers, and their toddlers. Early Child Res Q. 1997;12(1):81–98
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    20. ↵
      1. Lariviere J,
      2. Rennick JE
      . Parent picture-book reading to infants in the neonatal intensive care unit as an intervention supporting parent-infant interaction and later book reading. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2011;32(2):146–152pmid:21289497
      OpenUrlPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Fletcher KL,
      2. Reese E
      . Picture book reading with young children: a conceptual framework. Dev Rev. 2005;25(1):64–103
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    22. ↵
      1. Levin I,
      2. Aram D
      . Mother–child joint writing and storybook reading and their effects on kindergartners’ literacy: an intervention study. Read Writ. 2012;25(1):217–249
      OpenUrl
    23. ↵
      1. Clingenpeel BT,
      2. Pianta RC
      . Mothers’ sensitivity and book-reading interactions with first graders. Early Educ Dev. 2007;18(1):1–22
      OpenUrl
    24. ↵
      1. Mol SE,
      2. Bus AG,
      3. de Jong MT,
      4. Smeets DJH
      . Added value of dialogic parent–child book readings: a meta-analysis. Early Educ Dev. 2008;19(1):7–26
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    25. ↵
      1. Pellegrini AD,
      2. Galda L
      . Joint reading as a context: explicating the ways context is created by participants. In: van Kleeck A, Stahl SA, Bauer EB, eds. On Reading Books to Children: Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003:307–320
    26. ↵
      1. Bingham GE
      . Testing a Model of Parent-Child Relationships, Parent-Child Joint Book Reading, and Children’s Emergent Literacy Skills [dissertation]. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University; 2002
    27. ↵
      1. Williams KD
      . The Effects of an Interactive Reading Intervention on Early Literacy Development and Positive Parenting Interactions for Young Children of Teenage Mothers [dissertation]. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon; 2000
    28. ↵
      1. National Institute for Literacy
      . Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Panel. 2008. Available at: https://lincs.ed.gov/publications/pdf/NELPReport09.pdf. Accessed February 8, 2018
    29. ↵
      1. Zevenbergen AA,
      2. Whitehurst GJ
      . Dialogic reading: a shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In: van Kleeck A, Stahl SA, Bauer EB, eds. On Reading Books to Children: Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003:177–202
    30. ↵
      1. Gorwood P; The EGOFORS Initiative
      . W04-02–psychosocial functioning (PSF)–definition and measurement. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(suppl 1):1pmid:22153731
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    31. ↵
      1. Ro E
      . Conceptualization of Psychosocial Functioning: Understanding Structure and Relations With Personality and Psychopathology [doctoral thesis]. Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa; 2010
    32. ↵
      1. Chen SY
      . Social Support, Stress, and Depression: Measurement and Analysis of Social Well-Being, Mental Health, and Quality of Life in a Context of Aging and Chinese Culture [doctoral thesis]. Oakland, CA: University of California; 1996
    33. ↵
      1. Villalobos GH,
      2. Vargas AM,
      3. Rondón MA,
      4. Felknor SA
      . Design of psychosocial factors questionnaires: a systematic measurement approach. Am J Ind Med. 2013;56(1):100–110pmid:22628068
      OpenUrlPubMed
    34. ↵
      1. Roesch SC,
      2. Norman GJ,
      3. Merz EL,
      4. Sallis JF,
      5. Patrick K
      . Longitudinal measurement invariance of psychosocial measures in physical activity research: an application to adolescent data. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2013;43(4):721–729
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    35. ↵
      1. Peuskens J,
      2. Gorwood P; EGOFORS Initiative
      . How are we assessing functioning in schizophrenia? A need for a consensus approach. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(6):391–395pmid:21632218
      OpenUrlPubMed
    36. ↵
      1. Granner ML,
      2. Evans AE
      . Measurement properties of psychosocial and environmental measures associated with fruit and vegetable intake among middle school adolescents. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2012;44(1):2–11pmid:21852195
      OpenUrlPubMed
    37. ↵
      1. Kreitler S,
      2. Weyl Ben Arush M
      , eds. Psychosocial Aspects of Pediatric Oncology. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons; 2004
    38. ↵
      1. Moher D,
      2. Liberati A,
      3. Tetzlaff J,
      4. Altman DG; PRISMA Group
      . Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7):e1000097pmid:19621072
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    39. ↵
      1. Bus AG,
      2. van IJzendoorn MH,
      3. Pellegrini AD
      . Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Rev Educ Res. 1995;65(1):1–21
      OpenUrlCrossRef
      1. Mol SE,
      2. Bus AG
      . To read or not to read: a meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(2):267–296pmid:21219054
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Kim JS,
      2. Quinn DM
      . The effects of summer reading on low-income children’s literacy achievement from kindergarten to grade 8: a meta-analysis of classroom and home interventions. Rev Educ Res. 2013;83(3):386–431
      OpenUrl
    40. ↵
      1. van Steensel R,
      2. McElvany N,
      3. Kurvers J,
      4. Herppich S
      . How effective are family literacy programs? Results of a meta-analysis. Rev Educ Res. 2011;81(1):69–96
      OpenUrl
    41. ↵
      1. Chen M,
      2. Chan KL
      . Effects of parenting programs on child maltreatment prevention: a meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2016;17(1):88–104pmid:25573846
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    42. ↵
      1. Schulz KF,
      2. Altman DG,
      3. Moher D; CONSORT Group
      . CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ. 2010;340:c332pmid:20332509
      OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    43. ↵
      1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ,
      2. van IJzendoorn MH,
      3. Juffer F
      . Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(2):195–215pmid:12696839
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    44. ↵
      1. Borenstein M,
      2. Hedges LV,
      3. Higgins JPT,
      4. Rothstein HR
      . Introduction to Meta-Analysis. West Sussex, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons Ltd; 2009
    45. ↵
      1. Lipsey MW,
      2. Wilson DB
      . Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001
    46. ↵
      1. Egger M,
      2. Davey Smith G,
      3. Schneider M,
      4. Minder C
      . Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–634pmid:9310563
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    47. ↵
      1. Begg CB,
      2. Mazumdar M
      . Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50(4):1088–1101pmid:7786990
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    48. ↵
      1. Sterne JA,
      2. Egger M,
      3. Smith GD
      . Systematic reviews in health care: investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. BMJ. 2001;323(7304):101–105pmid:11451790
      OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    49. ↵
      1. Duval S,
      2. Tweedie R
      . Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–463pmid:10877304
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    50. ↵
      1. Auger A,
      2. Reich SM,
      3. Penner EK
      . The effect of baby books on mothers’ reading beliefs and reading practices. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35(4):337–346pmid:25264394
      OpenUrlPubMed
    51. ↵
      1. Bierman KL,
      2. Welsh JA,
      3. Heinrichs BS,
      4. Nix RL,
      5. Mathis ET
      . Helping Head Start parents promote their children’s kindergarten adjustment: the research-based developmentally informed parent program. Child Dev. 2015;86(6):1877–1891pmid:26494108
      OpenUrlPubMed
    52. ↵
      1. DeLoatche KJ,
      2. Bradley-Klug KL,
      3. Ogg J,
      4. Kromrey JD,
      5. Sundman-Wheat AN
      . Increasing parent involvement among Head Start families: a randomized control group study. Early Child Educ J. 2015;43(4):271–279
      OpenUrl
    53. ↵
      1. Golova N,
      2. Alario AJ,
      3. Vivier PM,
      4. Rodriguez M,
      5. High PC
      . Literacy promotion for Hispanic families in a primary care setting: a randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 1999;103(5 pt 1):993–997pmid:10224178
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    54. ↵
      1. Goldfeld S,
      2. Quach J,
      3. Nicholls R,
      4. Reilly S,
      5. Ukoumunne OC,
      6. Wake M
      . Four-year-old outcomes of a universal infant-toddler shared reading intervention: the Let’s Read trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(11):1045–1052pmid:23126396
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    55. ↵
      1. Heubner CE
      . Promoting toddlers’ language development through community-based intervention. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2000;21(5):513–535
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    56. ↵
      1. O’Hare L,
      2. Connolly P
      . A cluster randomised controlled trial of “Bookstart+”: a book gifting programme. J Child Serv. 2014;9(1):18–30
      OpenUrl
    57. ↵
      1. Ortiz C,
      2. Stowe RM,
      3. Arnold DH
      . Parental influence on child interest in shared picture book reading. Early Child Res Q. 2001;16(2):263–281
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    58. ↵
      1. Scott S,
      2. O’Connor TG,
      3. Futh A,
      4. Matias C,
      5. Price J,
      6. Doolan M
      . Impact of a parenting program in a high-risk, multi-ethnic community: the PALS trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(12):1331–1341pmid:20868373
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    59. ↵
      1. Scott S,
      2. Sylva K,
      3. Doolan M, et al
      . Randomised controlled trial of parent groups for child antisocial behaviour targeting multiple risk factors: the SPOKES project. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(1):48–57pmid:19732250
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Wake M,
      2. Tobin S,
      3. Levickis P, et al
      . Randomized trial of a population-based, home-delivered intervention for preschool language delay. Pediatrics. 2013;132(4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/132/4/e895pmid:24043276
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    60. ↵
      1. Wake M,
      2. Levickis P,
      3. Tobin S, et al
      . Two-year outcomes of a population-based intervention for preschool language delay: an RCT. Pediatrics. 2015;136(4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/136/4/e838pmid:26347428
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    61. ↵
      1. Carter AS,
      2. Briggs-Gowan MJ,
      3. Jones SM,
      4. Little TD
      . The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA): factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2003;31(5):495–514pmid:14561058
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    62. ↵
      1. Reynolds CR,
      2. Kamphaus RW
      . Behavior Assessment System for Children. 2nd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2004
    63. ↵
      1. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group
      . Initial impact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems: I. The high-risk sample. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999;67(5):631–647pmid:10535230
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    64. ↵
      1. Goodman R
      . The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–586pmid:9255702
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    65. ↵
      1. Taylor E,
      2. Schachar R,
      3. Thorley G,
      4. Wieselberg M
      . Conduct disorder and hyperactivity: I. Separation of hyperactivity and antisocial conduct in British child psychiatric patients. Br J Psychiatry. 1986;149(6):760–767pmid:3790875
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    66. ↵
      1. Boggs SR,
      2. Eyberg S,
      3. Reynolds LA
      . Concurrent validity of the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory. J Clin Child Psychol. 1990;19(1):75–78
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    67. ↵
      1. Varni JW,
      2. Burwinkle TM,
      3. Seid M,
      4. Skarr D
      . The PedsQL 4.0 as a pediatric population health measure: feasibility, reliability, and validity. Ambul Pediatr. 2003;3(6):329–341pmid:14616041
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    68. ↵
      1. Abidin RR
      . Parenting Stress Index. 3rd ed. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1990
    69. ↵
      1. Abidin RR
      . In: Zalaquett CP, Woods Lathan RJ, eds. Evaluating Stress: A Book of Resources, 1st ed. Lanham, MD: University Press of America; 1997:277–292
    70. ↵
      1. Beck AT,
      2. Steer RA
      . Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1993
    71. ↵
      1. Fantuzzo J,
      2. Tighe E,
      3. Childs S
      . Family involvement questionnaire: a multivariate assessment of family participation in early childhood education. J Educ Psychol. 2000;92(2):367–376
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    72. ↵
      1. Dreyer BP,
      2. Mendelsohn AL,
      3. Tamis-LeMonda CS
      . Assessing the child’s cognitive home environment through parental report; reliability and validity. Early Dev Parent. 1996;5(4):271–287
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    73. ↵
      1. Webster-Stratton C,
      2. Jamila Reid M,
      3. Stoolmiller M
      . Preventing conduct problems and improving school readiness: evaluation of the Incredible Years Teacher and Child Training Programs in high-risk schools. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(5):471–488pmid:18221346
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    74. ↵
      1. DeBaryshe BD,
      2. Binder JC
      . Development of an instrument for measuring parental beliefs about reading aloud to young children. Percept Mot Skills. 1994;78(suppl 3):1303–1311
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    75. ↵
      1. Cohen J
      . A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155–159pmid:19565683
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    76. ↵
      1. Landry SH,
      2. Smith KE,
      3. Swank PR,
      4. Zucker T,
      5. Crawford AD,
      6. Solari EF
      . The effects of a responsive parenting intervention on parent-child interactions during shared book reading. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(4):969–986pmid:22122475
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    77. ↵
      1. Schwartz JI
      . An observational study of mother/child and father/child interactions in story reading. J Res Child Educ. 2004;19(2):105–114
      OpenUrl
    78. ↵
      1. Genisio MH
      . Breaking barriers with books: a fathers’ book-sharing program from prison. J Adolesc Adult Lit. 1996;40(2):92–100
      OpenUrl
    79. ↵
      1. Scott A,
      2. van Bysterveldt A,
      3. McNeill B
      . The effectiveness of an emergent literacy intervention for teenage parents. Infants Young Child. 2016;29(1):53–70
      OpenUrl
    80. ↵
      1. Di Santo A
      . Promoting preschool literacy: a family literacy program for homeless mothers and their children. Child Educ. 2012;88(4):232–240
      OpenUrl
    81. ↵
      1. Saracho ON
      . Fathers’ and young children’s literacy experiences. Early Child Dev Care. 2008;178(7–8):837–852
      OpenUrl
    82. ↵
      1. Ng TK,
      2. Wong DFK
      . The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for Chinese people: a meta-analysis [published online ahead of print November 1, 2017]. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. doi:10.1177/0004867417741555pmid:29143541
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    83. ↵
      1. Rodenburg R,
      2. Wagner JL,
      3. Austin JK,
      4. Kerr M,
      5. Dunn DW
      . Psychosocial issues for children with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2011;22(1):47–54pmid:21705279
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    84. ↵
      1. Scholten L,
      2. Willemen AM,
      3. Napoleone E, et al
      . Moderators of the efficacy of a psychosocial group intervention for children with chronic illness and their parents: what works for whom? J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40(2):214–227pmid:25342644
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    85. ↵
      1. Davis E,
      2. Conroy R
      . Psychological interventions for parents of children and adolescents with chronic illness. J Paediatr Child Health. 2015;51(10):1036–1038pmid:26428422
      OpenUrlPubMed
    • Copyright © 2018 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    PreviousNext
    Back to top

    Advertising Disclaimer »

    In this issue

    Pediatrics
    Vol. 141, Issue 4
    1 Apr 2018
    • Table of Contents
    • Index by author
    View this article with LENS
    PreviousNext
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Psychosocial Effects of Parent-Child Book Reading Interventions: A Meta-analysis
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Request Permissions
    Article Alerts
    Log in
    You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
    Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
    Citation Tools
    Psychosocial Effects of Parent-Child Book Reading Interventions: A Meta-analysis
    Qian-Wen Xie, Celia H.Y. Chan, Qingying Ji, Cecilia L.W. Chan
    Pediatrics Apr 2018, 141 (4) e20172675; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-2675

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Share
    Psychosocial Effects of Parent-Child Book Reading Interventions: A Meta-analysis
    Qian-Wen Xie, Celia H.Y. Chan, Qingying Ji, Cecilia L.W. Chan
    Pediatrics Apr 2018, 141 (4) e20172675; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-2675
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    Print
    Download PDF
    Insight Alerts
    • Table of Contents

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Conclusions
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & Data
    • Info & Metrics
    • Comments

    Related Articles

    • No related articles found.
    • PubMed
    • Google Scholar

    Cited By...

    • Differences in Parent-Toddler Interactions With Electronic Versus Print Books
    • Google Scholar

    More in this TOC Section

    • Machine Learning for Child and Adolescent Health: A Systematic Review
    • Effects of Probiotics in Preterm Infants: A Network Meta-analysis
    • Adverse Childhood Experiences and Justice System Contact: A Systematic Review
    Show more Review Article

    Similar Articles

    Subjects

    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Psychosocial Issues
    • Journal Info
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Policies
    • Overview
    • Licensing Information
    • Authors/Reviewers
    • Author Guidelines
    • Submit My Manuscript
    • Open Access
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Librarians
    • Institutional Subscriptions
    • Usage Stats
    • Support
    • Contact Us
    • Subscribe
    • Resources
    • Media Kit
    • About
    • International Access
    • Terms of Use
    • Privacy Statement
    • FAQ
    • AAP.org
    • shopAAP
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
    • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
    • RSS
    American Academy of Pediatrics

    © 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics