Jump to comment:
- RE: Seeing Voices and Cochlear Implants
Geers and colleagues, in a meticulously executed study, reported a delay in speech acquisition in patients using sign language prior to cochlear implantation (1). We fear that this may discourage parents from using sign language with unintended negative consequences.
Hellen Keller describes how blindness only separates the blind from things, but deafness separates people from people (2). Oliver Sacks in his fascinating book,‘Seeing Voices’ describes the fate of the hearing impaired before 1780 when the “congenitally deaf or ‘deaf and dumb’ were considered ‘dumb’ (stupid)” (3). Without language they were cut off from fellow humans, culture and information, no matter how good their native capacities.
In the 1780s deaf schools manned by deaf teachers began to become popular and sign language came into its own. Soon after this we had “deaf writers, deaf engineers, deaf philosophers and deaf intellectuals” which was inconceivable previously.
According to Sacks the tide turned again in the 1870s when it became popular to think that sign language cut the deaf from the general population and that they should be taught lip reading and speaking. Sign language was proscribed. The teaching of speech took five to eight years of intensive tutoring leaving little time for transferring information, culture and anything else, such that child ended up ‘a functional illiterate who had, at best, a poor imitation of speech’(3).
Even for infants with hearing, infan...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - RE: The Big Picture: Overlooking the Long-Term Benefits of Bilingualism for Deaf Children
Even if research such as Geers et al. or other similar studies in the future might provide or appear to provide more definitive evidence that using sign language along with spoken language might cause some initial delays in acquiring either or both languages, well-designed long-term longitudinal studies of deaf children as they grow up into adults are necessary to fully ascertain the overall benefits of using spoken language and sign language with deaf children. Many recent studies have confirmed various benefits of bilingualism, including improving executive functioning in toddlers1 and delaying the onset of dementia in older adults2. While it is possible that teaching deaf children both sign language and spoken language might lead to slight lexical delays2 during their formative years, we should not overlook the long-term benefits of bilingual deaf adults who have stronger executive functioning1,2 and are more likely to be well-adjusted, being able to function and participate in both deaf and hearing communities.3 Therefore, I, as a profoundly deaf-since-birth developmental-behavioral pediatrician, strongly encourage future research of language development in deaf children to adopt a more long-term and holistic approach in understanding and appreciating the value of both spoken and sign languages for all deaf children rather than find ways to negate one or the other approach. From a deaf epistemology perspective3, there is truly no need to conduct and publish short-t...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - Evidence that use of sign language is NOT “distracting”
In response to the numerous criticisms of MS#2016-3489 (Early sign exposure and cochlear implant benefits), Geers et al. state “we investigated whether more focused spoken language exposure, free from distraction of manual signs, would provide a more attainable broad-based strategy for verbal language development.” Further, they explicitly cite my work to support the conclusion that, because of differences in the structure of ASL and English, “. . . conscientious and proficient use of ASL would detract from the amount of time spent stimulating and reinforcing spoken language development and could influence the structure of spoken language.” Nothing in the cited work remotely supports such a conclusion - in neither my 2002 book “Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from Sign Language Research” nor in the 2005 article on code-blending by hearing adult ASL-English bilinguals that Geers et al. cite. On the contrary, recent research clearly demonstrates that simultaneously perceiving signs and spoken words does not negatively impact spoken word recognition or learning in deaf children1 and facilitates word recognition in adults2. Further, the augmentative use of signs has recently been shown to be beneficial to word learning in deaf children3.
In addition to mis-citing the literature, the response does not address the “correlation is causation” fallacy brought up by several commentators. In the article, as well as in their response to comments, the authors stron...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - RE: Response to Comments on MS#2016-3489: Early Sign Exposure and Cochlear Implant Benefits
Response to Comments on “Early Sign Language Exposure and Cochlear Implant Benefits”
By: Ann E. Geersa, Ph.D, Christine M. Mitchellb, ScM, Andrea Warner-Czyza, Ph.D., Nae-Yu Wangb, Ph.D., Laurie S. Eisenbergc, Ph.D.
a Callier Center for Communication Disorders, University of Texas at Dallas
b Welch Center for Prevention, Epidemiology and Clinical Research, Johns Hopkins University
c Keck School of Medicine of the University of Southern California, Los Angeles
--------
1. Issues with Regard to Design of the Study
Sound scientific research evaluating the effects of an intervention is predicated on random selection or assignment to treatment and control groups. The Geers et al. (2017) study violates this basic tenet of research design
While we agree that randomly assigning families at the time of diagnosis to intervention programs that differ in their use of sign language would be an efficient method to address sign language benefits, this approach is impractical and unethical for a variety of reasons. A decision regarding a child’s communication mode arises from parental choice based on information available at the time the child becomes a candidate for cochlear implantation. The current observational cohort study examined more prospectively collected pre-implant d...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - Spoken Language Development with Cochlear Implants
The utility of sign language as a supplement to spoken language for children using cochlear implants is complicated to systematically study. This topic has been debated for many years and yet there remains a paucity of high quality data to help parents make decisions that may affect their child and family. The Geers et al article is an effort to further our understanding of this topic of interest. The data comes from a large and well-respected database, the methodology and analysis have satisfied the peer review process and thus the findings have appropriately been published. Like most good research, it raises many more excellent questions for future study and has generated discussion. This is a healthy and necessary process for objectively advancing our knowledge.
Children born deaf are a heterogenic population. This prospective study by Geers et al provides insight into a distinct population of children with cochlear implants. Specifically, the children studied were implanted at a young age, had no other disabilities, were managed at centers with specialized personnel, and had highly educated mothers. The children and their families received expert spoken language therapy, with or without varied amounts and types of sign language. In this population, supplemental sign language did not provide an advantage for the development of spoken language. A study using consistent approaches for sign language and randomization might provide more definitive insight but doing...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - RE: Responsible Publishing
A goal shared by all parents of congenitally deaf children is for their child to achieve language competencies that provide a foundation for nurturing, education, and engagement in society. Today, even against a backdrop of miraculous technological advances and an increasing sophistication in our understanding of human capacity for language acquisition, we fall short of being able to provide a prescription for individual deaf children which will guarantee spoken language competencies. In the absence of a definitive solution we must rely upon rigorous research to make correct inferences in ascribing best practices for parents with deaf children.
Show More
Sound scientific research evaluating the effects of an intervention is predicated on random selection or assignment to treatment and control groups. This ensures that each participant or subject has an equal chance of being placed in any group. Random assignment of participants helps to ensure that any differences between and within the groups are not systematic at the outset of the experiment. Thus, any differences between groups recorded at the end of the experiment can be more confidently attributed to the experimental procedures or treatment.
The Geers et al. (2017) study violates this basic tenet of research design. The incorrect assumption made by Geers et al. (2017) is that all of their subjects have an equal potential to develop auditory and spoken language skills, or more accurately that this potential will...Competing Interests: None declared. - Underlying Epistemologies—A Comment to Geers et al.
Regarding Geers et al.’s methodology, data analysis, and conclusions, we find general agreement with other comments, but here we focus on their underlying epistemology. Geer et al.’s epistemological approach focused on understanding how to help deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) children function within a hearing world. The scientific method, however, requires an understanding that science is not neutral – as has been noted over the past centuries with regards to other minority groups. If one accepts the goal of raising DHH children to pass as hearing (1), then the main focus on spoken language acquisition fits with the epistemology of a DHH child as having a disability. The disability epistemology that Geers et al. utilizes, however, accepts the high probability of curtailing and restricting the cognitive and linguistic potential of DHH children.
Cognitive ecology recognizes the importance of embodied cognition, meaning that one’s environment shapes or limits one’s cognition (2). These holistic views of a DHH child as one who is different – rather than one with a deficit – is supported by a Deaf epistemology (3). This perspective facilitates a supportive cognitive ecology with the deaf individual as a visual being. Given this difference, most – if not all – DHH individuals will benefit from an alignment of their cognitive ecology with their own sensory capabilities provided by Deaf epistemology.
From a Deaf epistemology framework, it is worthwhile to note refer...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - Failure to Distinguish Among Competing Hypotheses
The data presented by Geers et al. are consistent with three mutually-exclusive theories about the impact of natural sign languages on the development of English language skills: that natural sign languages (1) impede, (2) facilitate, or (3) have no impact on English development. Although Geers et al. clearly favor Theory 1, it is neither the only nor the best interpretation of their data.
Theory 2 is consistent with the data because although the authors quantified exposure to manual communication (including ASL), they did not measure proficiency in ASL. Children who have not acquired the grammar of ASL are not predicted to benefit from it as a foundation for subsequent mastery of English. We applaud these authors for considering variation in the amount of exposure to manual communication; however, we are dismayed to see ASL lumped together with other types of manual communication. Such coarse grouping prevents this crucial hypothesis from being adequately tested. (That is, children who are exposed mainly to English-based signing systems will not acquire the grammar of ASL; their performance is therefore uninformative about Theory 2.) Contrary to their claims, the authors have not falsified the theory that mastering the grammar of a sign language helps a child master the grammar of a spoken language.
Theory 3 is consistent with the data because observed differences might be attributable to other demographic factors that likely affected initial inclusion, at...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - Operationalization and Measurement of Sign Language
We outline a number of fundamental issues in how sign language exposure and proficiency were operationalized and reported by Geers et al. Most importantly, the authors did not distinguish between those exposed to American Sign Language (ASL) versus English signing systems (e.g., signing exact English, sign-supported English, baby sign) when classifying children. This is a fatal flaw because, in contrast to artificial English signing systems, natural sign languages such as ASL are legitimate languages – as long-affirmed by the Linguistic Society of America[1] – with all the cognitive benefits a natural language provides. The study is recklessly misleading because of this inappropriate conflation, especially given that the authors’ conclusions contribute to long-standing bias, resistance, and misperceptions against natural sign languages in clinical recommendations for deaf children.
Among other issues, there is not enough information provided about participants’ sign language proficiency and exposure. At minimum, it is critical to know the number of children exposed to only ASL (as opposed to artificial signing systems), the age of first exposure to ASL, the number of ASL language models, and the ASL proficiency of parents and children. Effects of ‘sign language exposure’ may have been carried by participants who used an artificial signing system, received late exposure relative to the critical period of language acquisition, had only one ASL model, and families with...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - Concerns with Data Analysis and Interpretation
Based upon an analysis of data collected from the CDaCI study, Geers et al. argue that parents’ use of sign-based communication systems may result in poorer listening and spoken language skills, and reading outcomes for deaf children who have received a CI. Their findings conflict with those reported by Niparko et al. (1), which used the same dataset, raising concerns about variable selection and selective reporting of analyses. Whereas Niparko et al. concluded that signing had no effect on spoken language outcomes as measured using the RDLS, Geers et al. argue that the CASL revealed poorer spoken language and listening outcomes for deaf children whose families have chosen visual communication strategies.
Additionally, they point out that those same children show long-term deficiencies in reading skills as measured by the WJ-IV. Given the large, multivariate CDaCI dataset and the need for robustness in the findings across a range of predictor and outcome measures, we argue that these differences may reflect measurement error. Two data points do not allow for determination of developmental change (2), therefore the data cannot be classified as truly longitudinal. In addition, the data are presented in a grouped format, so actual change for individual children is obscured. The grouped format furthermore exhibits high variability which occludes some of the conclusions made by Geers et al.
Finally, Geers et al report no difference in outcomes for children in fam...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - RE: Methodological Concerns Suspend Interpretations
Geers et al. contains significant methodological issues that should moderate any findings and claims of sign language’s role in implanted deaf children’s spoken and written English development.
First, the study sample is (understandably) non-randomized; thus categorization factors may be related to outcomes. Asserting a causal conclusion from a correlational non-randomized study is inappropriate – especially when a simpler explanation may exist: parents of deaf children who are not progressing with their CI may be more likely to begin, or continue, signing with their child. This would imply that poor oral outcomes encourage use of signing, rather than the use of signing limiting oral outcomes.
Secondly, while the authors reported no statistically significant differences between groups at baseline, the actual data suggest clinically significant differences that were statistically non-significant due to small group sizes. Multi-layered and complex variables such as maternal education (69% vs 50%), income <$50k (32% vs 43%) and age of onset (0.3m v 1.2m) are well-known to influence language and reading outcomes (it is also unclear if age of onset is actually age of diagnosis). Additionally, auditory perception abilities at baseline were much lower in the group that continued to sign; indeed, the authors recognize early speech recognition predicts later speech intelligibility. Furthermore, type and frequency of post-implant rehabilitation – an educational exp...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared. - To The Editor: Concerns With Correlative Data
We were pleased to see a study on speech and language outcomes in deaf children with cochlear implants (CI) was published in the June issue of Pediatrics. Geers and co-authors provided a framework for one of the largest studies of this kind to date.
However, it would benefit your readership to understand significant concerns that were raised in examining this data. The study reports associations between use of sign language in the home and reduced speech and language outcomes. We strongly question the interpretation of the data. The study cohorts were derived from subjective parental self-report of sign language use in the home, that included a variety of visual communication tools (“Signed Exact English, sign support”, etc) in addition to formalized sign language. While this article reports a correlation between exposure to signing and delayed spoken language and reading outcomes, the interpreted conclusions state causal relations that the study methodology fails to establish. The conclusions drawn exclusively from correlations, therefore, must be viewed with caution. The findings beg that more data be gathered, factors accounted for, and carefully constructed studies conducted and analyzed.
We would have liked to see a more robust discussion regarding why the authors reached the conclusion that “there was no advantage to parents’ use of sign language either before or after CI,” when it is contrary to a body of published peer-reviewed data regard...
Show MoreCompeting Interests: None declared.