Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Paternal Stimulation and Early Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries

Joshua Jeong, Dana Charles McCoy, Aisha K. Yousafzai, Carmel Salhi and Günther Fink
Pediatrics October 2016, 138 (4) e20161357; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1357
Joshua Jeong
aDepartment of Global Health and Population, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dana Charles McCoy
bGraduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Aisha K. Yousafzai
cDepartment of Paediatrics and Child Health, Aga Khan University, Karachi, Pakistan; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Carmel Salhi
dDepartment of Health Sciences, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Günther Fink
aDepartment of Global Health and Population, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Supplemental
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Few studies have examined the relationship between paternal stimulation and children’s growth and development, particularly in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs). This study aimed to estimate the prevalence of paternal stimulation and to assess whether paternal stimulation was associated with early child growth and development.

METHODS: Data from the Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys rounds 4 and 5 were combined across 38 LMICs. The sample comprised 87 286 children aged 3 and 4 years. Paternal stimulation was measured by the number of play and learning activities (up to 6) a father engaged in with his child over the past 3 days. Linear regression models were used to estimate standardized mean differences in height-for-age z-scores and Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI) z-scores across 3 levels of paternal stimulation, after controlling for other caregivers’ stimulation and demographic covariates.

RESULTS: A total of 47.8% of fathers did not engage in any stimulation activities, whereas 6.4% of fathers engaged in 5 or 6 stimulation activities. Children whose fathers were moderately engaged in stimulation (1–4 activities) showed ECDI scores that were 0.09 SD (95% confidence interval [CI]: –0.12 to –0.06) lower than children whose fathers were highly engaged; children whose fathers were unengaged showed ECDI scores that were 0.14 SD lower (95% CI: –0.17 to –0.12). Neither moderate paternal stimulation nor lack of paternal stimulation was associated with height-for-age z-scores, relative to high stimulation.

CONCLUSION Increasing paternal engagement in stimulation is likely to improve early child development in LMICs.

  • Abbreviations:
    CI —
    confidence interval
    ECD —
    early child development
    ECDI —
    early childhood development index
    ECE —
    early childhood education
    HAZ —
    height-for-age z-score
    LMICs —
    low- and middle-income countries
    MICS —
    Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey
    MICS5 —
    Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey round 5
    MICS4 —
    Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey round 4
  • What’s Known on This Subject:

    The effects of maternal stimulation on early child development (ECD) have been well-documented. Few studies have examined paternal engagement in stimulation in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs); even less is known regarding the influences of paternal stimulation on ECD outcomes.

    What This Study Adds:

    This study is one of the first to show that nearly half of fathers in LMICs do not engage in stimulation activities with their children and that lower levels of paternal stimulation are associated with poorer ECD outcomes.

    More than 200 million children <5 years across low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) are estimated to not reach their full developmental potential due to malnutrition, inadequate stimulation, and other risk factors associated with poverty.1 Parenting interventions have become increasingly prioritized as a key strategy for mitigating such risk factors and increasing children’s resilience during early childhood.2–5 A considerable body of evidence has demonstrated moderate-to-large effect sizes for responsive parenting interventions on various aspects of early child development (ECD), including cognitive and socioemotional development6–9 and early growth and nutrition.10–12

    The majority of the ECD literature and ECD programing efforts, however, has focused on mothers. Although positive mother–child interactions and secure maternal attachment are important,13,14 the strong focus on mother–child dyads has resulted in limited attention to the role of other caregivers, and particularly fathers, in supporting children’s development.

    Over the past several decades, the role of men in families has evolved because of demographic, socioeconomic, and cultural transitions.15 For example, greater participation of women in the workforce and large-scale labor migration have resulted in men taking on shared family responsibilities with their female partners, including parenting and engaging in support for their children’s healthy development.15 The importance of paternal involvement on children’s wellbeing has been explored mainly in high-income countries.16 A variety of ways have been discussed, ranging from physically stimulating play17; decision making about health care and education15; coparenting with other caregivers to complement, strengthen, or compensate for each other18,19; and providing financial resources that can in turn improve nutritional care, hygiene, and the overall home environment.20,21

    Studies from high-income countries have furthermore demonstrated strong associations between paternal involvement and various areas of development, including improvements in young children’s nutrition20; early cognitive22–24 and socio-emotional skills25–28; increased language development24,29; reduced child problem behaviors26; and reduced rates of child neglect.30 However, to date, few studies have examined the associations between paternal involvement and ECD in LMICs. Paternal stimulation may be an important protective factor in LMICs where children continue to be exposed to a host of risk factors.

    In this paper, we aimed to describe the prevalence of paternal stimulation in LMICs and to understand whether different levels of paternal stimulation are associated with ECD. In particular, we defined paternal stimulation based on a set of cognitively and psychosocially enriching activities. We defined ECD broadly by examining physical growth and a composite index of basic learning, socioemotional, physical, and literacy–numeracy skills. These 2 outcomes were examined separately given the growing evidence that these processes are only mildly correlated.31

    Methods

    Data

    The Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS) program is a nationally representative and internationally standardized household survey program developed by United Nations Children’s Fund that captures information about children in LMICs.32 Beginning with the MICS round 4 (MICS4) in 2010, the Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI) was introduced to the ECD module of the questionnaire for children under 5. We combined all nationally representative MICS4 and MICS round 5 (MICS5) surveys that included the ECDI and caregiver stimulation questions and were publically available before March 2016. Figure 1 illustrates the geographic coverage of the pooled data set.

    FIGURE 1
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 1

    Map of 38 countries included in this analysis.

    Measures

    Child Growth and Development

    Physical growth and child development were examined separately as 2 related but independent outcome measures of early child wellbeing. Children’s heights were converted to height-for-age z-scores (HAZ) using the World Health Organization 2006 growth standards.33 We analyzed both the continuous HAZ variable and a binary indicator for stunting (HAZ < –2 SD) as an indicator of growth faltering.

    Child development was measured among children aged 36 to 59 months using the caregiver-reported ECDI.34,35 The 10 items of the ECDI were established through consultation among child development experts and based on the results of multistage and multicountry pilot tests, measurement analyses, and validation studies.36 The 10 dichotomous (yes/no) ECDI items pertain to 4 domains of development, literacy–numeracy, physical, socioemotional, and learning, which have been previously validated across a large number of LMICs using confirmatory factor analysis.37 (Supplemental Table 4 provides details on the ECDI). Several items of the ECDI are directly comparable to items found in well-validated ECD measures, such as the Ages and Stages Questionnaire and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire37; furthermore, the ECDI has been used in other recent studies.37,38 A composite score, ranging from 0 to 10, was created by summing the number of positive responses. The internal consistency of the ECDI in this sample was fair (α = 0.55). The ECDI total sum score was normalized to a mean of 0 and a SD of 1 so as to be directly comparable to the standardized scale of HAZ. ECDI was also dichotomized to categorize children with low developmental scores, or 1 SD below the mean score in the sample.

    Paternal Stimulation

    Primary caregivers were asked to report on whether mothers, fathers, and/or other household member were engaged in any of the following 6 activities with their children in the past 3 days: (1) reading books or looking at pictures; (2) telling stories; (3) singing songs; (4) taking the child outside; (5) playing with the child; (6) naming, counting, or drawing with the child. These items reflect a measure of caregivers’ engagement in stimulation to support ECD.39 A summary score was created, which ranged from 0 (no paternal engagement in any stimulation activity) to 6 (paternal engagement in all stimulation activities within the last 3 days), similarly to how it has been described and used as a measure of caregiving and stimulation in other studies.39–41 In this sample, paternal stimulation showed good internal consistency (α = 0.77). The total number of stimulation activities was categorized into 3 groups: high engagement (5–6 activities), moderate engagement (1–4 activities), or no engagement (0 activities). This classification was motivated by a primary interest in examining differences among fathers who engaged in none of the stimulation activities, as compared with those who engaged in some or all of the activities.

    Other Covariates

    To reduce the risk of confounding, we included the following covariates: maternal and other caregiver’s stimulation; paternal and maternal education and marital status; maternal age; age (months) and sex of the child; ECE attendance; urban or rural residence; and household wealth quintile. Maternal and other caregiver’s stimulation were captured by using the same 6 items as paternal stimulation. Caregivers’ level of education was categorized into 4 groups: no education or incomplete primary education, completed primary education, completed secondary education, or tertiary education. The wealth index variable included in the public-release MICS data set was calculated based on the first principal component of a group of context-specific assets owned by the household and divided into quintiles within each country: poorest, poor, middle, rich, richest.42

    Analysis

    Separate unadjusted and adjusted linear regression models were estimated to examine the associations between levels of paternal stimulation and HAZ and ECDI z-scores. All models included country and survey year fixed effects to minimize any confounding through differences in survey methodology and over time. Adjusted models included the aforementioned set of covariates, which were determined a priori.

    An additional set of models was tested that included 2-way interactions between paternal stimulation and any significant predictor of the adjusted model to examine the presence of effect modification in the sample. If any interaction term was statistically significant at P < .05, analyses were repeated for subgroups stratified by that predictor. Lastly, to assess robustness of results for paternal stimulation on child growth and development, stunting and low ECDI score were substituted for HAZ and ECDI z-score, respectively, in all models and rerun with logistic regression. Additionally, associations between paternal stimulation and the 4 subdomains of the ECDI were explored.

    All children aged 36 to 59 months were included in the sample if they had information on either height-for-age or the ECDI measure and for the parental stimulation questions. The sample was restricted to children whose biological fathers were living in the household. Missing data were examined and assumed to be missing at random given the significant result of Little’s Missing Completely at Random test at the P < .001 level (indicating that the data are not Missing Completely at Random)43 and that the “missingness” of the data were predicted by other observed variables. Missing covariate values were estimated using separate multiple imputation regression models, including all other covariates as predictors. On substituting predicted values for missing values, adjusted models additionally included dummy variables that were coded 1 if data in that original covariate were missing, thereby retaining the total sample for the adjusted analyses and minimizing bias in estimation.44

    Statistical analyses were conducted in Stata version 13.1 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX). All estimates represent unweighted in-sample relationships; to adjust for the complex survey design of MICS, SEs were clustered at the country enumeration area level.

    Results

    A total of 87 286 child records were available across 38 countries (Supplemental Table 5). Sociodemographic and ECD-related characteristics for the sample are presented in Table 1. The average age of the child was 47.2 months (SD = 6.9). Approximately 29.7% of mothers reported no formal education, and 21.7% of fathers reported no formal education. Nearly all caregivers (97.8%) reported being married or in a union. The mean age of mothers was 30.6 years (SD = 6.5; range, 15–49). The majority of the sample lived in rural settings (63.5%).

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 1

    Sample Characteristics (n = 87 286)

    In total, 6.4% of fathers were highly engaged in stimulation activities (5 or 6 activities); 45.7% of fathers were moderately engaged (1–4 activities); and 47.8% of fathers did not perform any of the stimulation activities with their children in the previous 3 days. The proportion of fathers unengaged in any stimulation activities varied widely across countries, ranging from 11.3% in Serbia to 78.4% in Swaziland (Supplemental Table 6). On average, fathers who were highly engaged in stimulation were more educated, more wealthy, from urban areas, and had partners who were also more educated and engaged in stimulation activities (Supplemental Table 7).

    Mean HAZ in the sample was –1.41 (SD = 1.55), ranging from a mean of –2.17 in Laos to 0.41 in Bosnia; prevalence of stunting ranged from 54.9% in Laos to 1.6% in St. Lucia. Children’s average development score was 5.76 out of 10 (SD = 1.89). Average raw ECDI scores ranged from 4.35 in Chad to 8.43 in Barbados (Supplemental Table 6).

    Relationships Between Levels of Paternal Stimulation and HAZ and ECDI Scores

    Unadjusted and adjusted results for levels of paternal stimulation on HAZ and ECDI z-scores are presented in Table 2. In the unadjusted model, both moderate paternal stimulation (β = –0.17; 95% confidence interval [CI], –0.22 to –0.11; P < .001) and lack of paternal stimulation (β = –0.30; 95% CI, –0.35 to –0.25; P < .001) were negatively associated with HAZ relative to high stimulation. In the fully adjusted model, these associations were smaller and not statistically significant (β = 0.02; 95% CI, –0.03 to 0.07; P = .33 for moderate, and β = –0.02; 95% CI, –0.07 to 0.03; P = .42 for lack of stimulation).

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 2

    Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations (95% CI) Between Levels of Paternal Stimulation and HAZ and ECDI z-Score

    For ECDI z-score, moderate paternal stimulation was associated with a 0.26 SD lower ECDI z-score in unadjusted models (β = –0.26; 95% CI, –0.29 to –0.23; P < .001) and no paternal stimulation was associated with a 0.40 SD lower ECDI score (β = –0.40; 95% CI, –0.43 to –0.38; P < .001), as compared with high paternal stimulation. In the fully adjusted model, these associations were smaller but remained statistically significant: children whose fathers were moderately engaged in stimulation had a 0.09 SD lower ECDI score (β = –0.09; 95% CI, –0.12 to –0.06; P < .001); and children whose fathers were completely unengaged in stimulation had a 0.14 SD lower ECDI score (β = –0.14; 95% CI, –0.17 to –0.12; P < .001), as compared with children whose fathers were highly engaged. Figure 2 shows the dose-response relationships between paternal stimulation activities and HAZ and ECDI z-scores. Overall, the adjusted relationship between paternal stimulation and ECDI score is almost perfectly linear, whereas such a relationship is undetectable for HAZ.

    FIGURE 2
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 2

    Adjusted relationships between decreasing number of stimulation activities performed by the father and HAZ and ECDI z-score.

    Subgroup Analyses

    Two-way interaction terms between paternal stimulation and each covariate of the adjusted model were tested to examine whether associations between paternal stimulation and ECDI z-score and/or HAZ differed across these other well-documented risk factors. Two interaction terms were significant predictors of ECDI z-scores: the interaction term between paternal stimulation and maternal stimulation (P < .001) and that between paternal stimulation and ECE (P < .001). None of the interaction terms were significant in the models predicting HAZ. The results of the subgroup analyses for ECDI z-scores are presented in Table 3.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 3

    Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations (95% CI) Between Levels of Paternal Stimulation and ECDI z-Score by Levels of Maternal Stimulation and Children’s ECE Attendance

    Greater associations between paternal stimulation and ECDI scores were seen when there were low levels of maternal stimulation. The negative associations of paternal stimulation among children whose mothers were unengaged (moderate paternal stimulation: β = –0.25; 95% CI, –0.34 to –0.15; P < .001; and no paternal stimulation: β = –.33; 95% CI: –0.43 to –0.24; P < .001) were nearly three-fold larger in magnitude than among children whose mothers were highly engaged in stimulation (moderate paternal stimulation: β = –0.08; 95% CI, –0.11 to –0.05; P < .001; and no paternal stimulation: β = –0.11; 95% CI, –0.15 to –0.07; P < .001).

    Greater associations of paternal stimulation were also seen when children did not attend ECE. The negative associations of paternal stimulation among children who did not attend ECE (moderate paternal stimulation: β = –0.11; 95% CI, –0.15 to –0.07; P < .001; and no paternal stimulation: β = –0.16; 95% CI, –0.20 to –0.13; P < .001) were larger in magnitude in comparison with children who attended ECE (moderate paternal stimulation: β = –0.08; 95% CI, –0.12 to –0.05; P < .001; and no paternal stimulation: β = –0.13; 95% CI, –0.17 to –0.09; P < .001); however, the differences between subgroups by ECE attendance were not statistically significant.

    Robustness Checks

    To examine the robustness of results, HAZ and ECDI z-scores in all primary models were dichotomized and substituted as stunting and low ECDI scores, respectively. The results of these alternative classifications (Supplemental Table 8) corroborated our significant findings between levels of paternal stimulation and ECDI scores and null results for growth, as previously presented in Tables 2 and 3.

    Adjusted associations between paternal stimulation and each of the subdomains of the ECDI were also examined. Moderate and no paternal stimulation were most strongly associated with literacy–numeracy (β = –0.11; 95% CI, –0.14 to –0.07 and β = –0.16; 95% CI, –0.19 to –0.13, respectively; Supplemental Table 9). Smaller associations were seen between moderate and no paternal stimulation and learning (β =–0.03; 95% CI, –0.06 to –0.01 and β = –0.06; 95% CI, –0.09 to –0.03) and socioemotional domains (β = –0.05; 95% CI, –0.07 and –0.03 and β = –0.06; 95% CI: –0.09 and –0.04). However, for the physical domain, associations were null for both levels of paternal stimulation.

    Discussion

    In this paper, we used nationally representative data from 38 countries to estimate the prevalence of paternal stimulation and examine the associations between paternal stimulation and ECD in LMICs. Our analysis has 2 main findings. First, paternal stimulation remains limited in the majority of sampled LMICs. In the total sample, 47.8% of fathers were completely unengaged in stimulation activities with their 3- and 4-year-old children, which is twice the proportion of mothers who were unengaged (24.5%). This two-fold difference between prevalence of maternal and paternal stimulation is consistent with findings of the only other study known to the authors that has previously described maternal and paternal stimulation caregiving practices in LMICs.40

    Second, lower levels of paternal stimulation appear to have small but statistically significant negative associations with child development, even after controlling for various potentially confounding characteristics. Our results indicate that, relative to a highly engaged father, having a moderately engaged father is associated with 0.09 SD lower ECDI scores, whereas having an unengaged father is associated with 0.14 SD lower ECDI scores. Although smaller in magnitude, these paternal stimulation effect sizes are consistent with the effect sizes seen for levels of maternal stimulation; they are also comparable in magnitude to the effect sizes of maternal primary school completion or an increase in wealth from the poorest to middle/rich quintile (Supplemental Table 10).

    Results from subgroup analyses suggest that the associations between paternal stimulation and ECDI scores are more pronounced for children whose mothers were unengaged in stimulation and for children who did not attend ECE. These findings not only highlight the importance of having at least 1 engaged caregiver who can provide stimulating opportunities, but also suggest that engaged fathers might offer an important opportunity for buffering against high-risk environments of inadequate enrichment. Our results extend previous research in LMICs by revealing interactive and compensating effects of 1 parent for the lower engagement of the other that are similar to findings reported in high-income countries.45,46 More generally, our findings directly link to the broader resilience literature that underscores how responsive and stimulating parenting protects children from the adverse effects of early and cumulative risk factor exposures on ECD outcomes.47–50

    Results also revealed small associations between paternal stimulation and all developmental subdomain of the ECDI, except the physical domain. These findings suggest that paternal stimulation activities, such as reading, counting, and playing, may be particularly important for reinforcing children’s ability to identify letters and numbers, to be independent and focused learners, and to engage well with others.

    No associations were revealed between paternal stimulation and HAZ. Although HAZ has been commonly used as proxy measures of ECD, our results underscore important distinctions between the ECDI measure and HAZ that should be considered when examining relationships between stimulation and ECD. More research is needed to understand the specific mechanisms between paternal stimulation and ECD versus growth. Finally, although our results suggest no associations between paternal stimulation and HAZ, additional paternal roles, such as responsive care, decision-making around children’s diet, or providing a safe home environment, should be examined to understand more broadly how fathers might influence child growth and ECD. Overall, future research should consider a more holistic perspective on the role of fathers in ECD.

    Several limitations of this study are noted. First, the MICS data are cross-sectional and observational, which means that causality of the relationship between paternal stimulation and ECDI scores cannot be established . Second, both the stimulation measure and the ECDI relied on the same primary caregivers’ self-report, which raises the possibility of recall bias and social desirability bias. Furthermore, the measure of stimulation only focuses on support for learning and does not capture duration or quality of paternal interactions or reflect other meaningful roles of fathers, such as financial investments in the child, physical touch, and culturally specific engagement approaches.51

    In addition, despite the fact that the ECDI has provided some of the first population-level estimates of ECD across LMICs that are based on specific developmental behaviors and skills, it is also limited in important ways. This 10-item measure was designed to be brief enough to administer within the existing and comprehensive MICS household survey program, but also broad enough to enable valid international comparisons. Although several items are found in well-validated measures of development (eg, ECDI item 13, “Does your child follow simple directions on how to do something correctly?” is also in the Ages and Stages Questionnaire for children aged 36–60 months), the ECDI is limited in its ability to fully capture children’s overall developmental capacities. Additionally, the ECDI showed low internal consistency in this study, a limitation that likely reflects the broad range of constructs it attempts to capture. To fully understand the nuances of the associations between stimulation and child outcomes, future research is needed using measures of ECD that are more domain-, age-, and culturally specific, as well as reliable for use across heterogeneous contexts within and across LMICs. Finally, our results are limited to biological fathers who were also living in the household and do not reflect other important male caregivers, such as nonbiological fathers or nonresident fathers, who may still be engaged in stimulation and their children’s early development.52

    Conclusions

    This study highlights how paternal stimulation remains severely limited in LMICs and how the lack of paternal stimulation has negative associations with developmental outcomes. ECD research, programs, and policies that reflect and target only maternal stimulation may overlook important influences of fathers. A deeper understanding of the relationship between paternal stimulation and children's early development and approaches to encouraging paternal engagement in children’s learning, development, and health are needed to most effectively ensure that children in LMICs reach their full developmental potential.

    Acknowledgments

    We thank the United Nations Children’s Fund and individual countries for collecting the MICS data.

    Footnotes

      • Accepted July 1, 2016.
    • Address correspondence to Joshua Jeong, MPH, Department of Global Health and Population, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, 665 Huntington Ave, 11th floor, Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: jjeong{at}mail.harvard.edu
    • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

    • FUNDING: No external funding.

    • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

    • COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2016-2456.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. Grantham-McGregor S,
      2. Cheung YB,
      3. Cueto S,
      4. Glewwe P,
      5. Richter L,
      6. Strupp B; International Child Development Steering Group
      . Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60–70pmid:17208643
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    2. ↵
      1. Walker SP,
      2. Wachs TD,
      3. Grantham-McGregor S, et al
      . Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378(9799):1325–1338pmid:21944375
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Engle PL,
      2. Fernald LC,
      3. Alderman H, et al; Global Child Development Steering Group
      . Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2011;378(9799):1339–1353pmid:21944378
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Aboud FE,
      2. Yousafzai AK
      . Global health and development in early childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:433–457pmid:25196276
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    3. ↵
      1. Daelmans B,
      2. Black MM,
      3. Lombardi J, et al; steering committee of a new scientific series on early child development
      . Effective interventions and strategies for improving early child development. BMJ. 2015;351:h4029pmid:26371213
      OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    4. ↵
      1. Chang SM,
      2. Grantham-McGregor SM,
      3. Powell CA, et al
      . Integrating a parenting intervention with routine primary health care: a cluster randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2015;136(2):272–280pmid:26148947
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Eshel N,
      2. Daelmans B,
      3. de Mello MC,
      4. Martines J
      . Responsive parenting: interventions and outcomes. Bull World Health Organ. 2006;84(12):991–998pmid:17242836
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Hamadani JD,
      2. Huda SN,
      3. Khatun F,
      4. Grantham-McGregor SM
      . Psychosocial stimulation improves the development of undernourished children in rural Bangladesh. J Nutr. 2006;136(10):2645–2652pmid:16988140
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    5. ↵
      1. Yousafzai AK,
      2. Rasheed MA,
      3. Rizvi A,
      4. Armstrong R,
      5. Bhutta ZA
      . Effect of integrated responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions in the Lady Health Worker programme in Pakistan on child development, growth, and health outcomes: a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet. 2014;384(9950):1282–1293pmid:24947106
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    6. ↵
      1. Powell C,
      2. Baker-Henningham H,
      3. Walker S,
      4. Gernay J,
      5. Grantham-McGregor S
      . Feasibility of integrating early stimulation into primary care for undernourished Jamaican children: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2004;329(7457):89pmid:15217841
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Vazir S,
      2. Engle P,
      3. Balakrishna N, et al
      . Cluster-randomized trial on complementary and responsive feeding education to caregivers found impr-oved dietary intake, growth and development among rural Indian toddlers. Matern Child Nutr. 2013;9(1):99–117pmid:22625182
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    7. ↵
      1. Aboud FE,
      2. Akhter S
      . A cluster-randomized evaluation of a responsive stimulation and feeding intervention in bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5). Available at:www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/127/5/e1191pmid:21502222
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    8. ↵
      1. Bowlby J
      . Attachment and Loss. Vol 1. New York: Basic Books; 1969.
    9. ↵
      1. Ainsworth MD
      . Infant--mother attachment. Am Psychol. 1979;34(10):932–937pmid:517843
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    10. ↵
      1. Richter L,
      2. Chikovore J,
      3. Makusha T, et al
      . Fatherhood and families. In: Men in families and family policy in a changing world. Available at: www.un.org/esa/socdev/family/docs/men-in-families.pdf. Accessed April 13, 2016
    11. ↵
      1. Lamb ME
      . The Role of the Father in Child Development. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2010
    12. ↵
      1. Lewis C,
      2. Lamb ME
      . Fathers’ influences on children’s development: The evidence from two-parent families. Eur J Psychol Educ. 2003;18(2):211–228
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    13. ↵
      1. Cabrera NJ,
      2. Fitzgerald HE,
      3. Bradley RH,
      4. Roggman L
      . The ecology of father-child relationships: An expanded model. J Fam Theory Rev. 2014;6(4):336–354
      OpenUrl
    14. ↵
      1. McHale JP,
      2. Lindahl KM
      , eds. Coparenting: A Conceptual and Clinical Examination of Family Systems. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2011
    15. ↵
      1. Dearden K,
      2. Crookston B,
      3. Madanat H,
      4. West J,
      5. Penny M,
      6. Cueto S
      . What difference can fathers make? Early paternal absence compromises Peruvian children’s growth. Matern Child Nutr. 2013;9(1):143–154pmid:22023325
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    16. ↵
      1. Jarrett RL
      . Living poor: Family life among single parent, African-American women. Soc Probl. 1994;41(1):30–49
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    17. ↵
      1. Black MM,
      2. Dubowitz H,
      3. Starr RH Jr
      . African American fathers in low income, urban families: development, behavior, and home environment of their three-year-old children. Child Dev. 1999;70(4):967–978pmid:10446729
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network
      . Child Care and Child Development: Results From the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. New York, New York: Guilford Press; 2005
    18. ↵
      1. Tamis-LeMonda CS,
      2. Shannon JD,
      3. Cabrera NJ,
      4. Lamb ME
      . Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Dev. 2004;75(6):1806–1820pmid:15566381
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    19. ↵
      1. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network
      . Fathers’ and mothers’ parenting behavior and beliefs as predictors of children’s social adjustment in the transition to school. J Fam Psychol. 2004;18(4):628–638pmid:15598168
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    20. ↵
      1. Ramchandani PG,
      2. Domoney J,
      3. Sethna V,
      4. Psychogiou L,
      5. Vlachos H,
      6. Murray L
      . Do early father-infant interactions predict the onset of externalising behaviours in young children? Findings from a longitudinal cohort study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(1):56–64pmid:22808985
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. DeKlyen M,
      2. Biernbaum MA,
      3. Speltz ML,
      4. Greenberg MT
      . Fathers and preschool behavior problems. Dev Psychol. 1998;34(2):264–275pmid:9541779
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Kvalevaag AL,
      2. Ramchandani PG,
      3. Hove O,
      4. Assmus J,
      5. Eberhard-Gran M,
      6. Biringer E
      . Paternal mental health and socioemotional and behavioral development in their children. Pediatrics. 2013;131(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/131/2/e463pmid:23296445
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    22. ↵
      1. Bronte‐Tinkew J,
      2. Ryan S,
      3. Carrano J,
      4. Moore KA
      . Resident fathers’ pregnancy intentions, prenatal behaviors, and links to involvement with infants. J Marriage Fam. 2007;69(4):977–990
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    23. ↵
      1. Dubowitz H,
      2. Black MM,
      3. Kerr MA,
      4. Starr RH Jr,
      5. Harrington D
      . Fathers and child neglect. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2000;154(2):135–141pmid:10665599
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    24. ↵
      1. Sudfeld CR,
      2. McCoy DC,
      3. Danaei G, et al
      . Linear growth and child development in low- and middle-income countries: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;135(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/135/5/e1266pmid:25847806
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    25. ↵
      1. UNICEF
      . Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey Manual, 2005: Monitoring the Situation of Children and Women. New York: UNICEF; 2005
    26. ↵
      1. WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Grouup;
      2. de Onis M
      . WHO Child Growth Standards based on length/height, weight and age. Acta Paediatr. 2006;95(suppl S450):76–85
      OpenUrl
    27. ↵
      1. UNICEF
      . ECD Monitoring. Available at: www.unicef.org/earlychildhood/index_69846.html. Accessed April 13, 2016
    28. ↵
      1. UNICEF
      . MICS4 Tools: Designing the Questionnaires. Available at: http://mics.unicef.org/tools?round=mics4. Accessed April 13, 2016
    29. ↵
      1. UNICEF
      . The Formative Years: UNICEF’s Work on Measuring Early Childhood Development. Available at: http://data.unicef.org/corecode/uploads/document6/uploaded_pdfs/corecode/Measuring-ECD-Brochure-HR-10_8_116.pdf. Accessed April 13, 2016
    30. ↵
      1. McCoy DC,
      2. Peet ED,
      3. Ezzati M, et al
      . Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: National, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002034pmid:27270467
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    31. ↵
      1. Miller AC,
      2. Murray MB,
      3. Thomson DR,
      4. Arbour MC
      . How consistent are associations between stunting and child development? Evidence from a meta-analysis of associations between stunting and multidimensional child development in fifteen low-and middle-income countries. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(8):1339–1347pmid:26355426
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    32. ↵
      1. Bornstein MH,
      2. Putnick DL
      . Cognitive and socioemotional caregiving in developing countries. Child Dev. 2012;83(1):46–61pmid:22277006
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    33. ↵
      1. Sun J,
      2. Liu Y,
      3. Chen EE,
      4. Rao N,
      5. Liu H
      . Factors related to parents’ engagement in cognitive and socio-emotional caregiving in developing countries: Results from Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey 3. Early Child Res Q. 2016;36:21–31
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    34. ↵
      1. Bornstein MH,
      2. Putnick DL,
      3. Lansford JE,
      4. Deater-Deckard K,
      5. Bradley RH
      . A developmental analysis of caregiving modalities across infancy in 38 low- and middle-income countries. Child Dev. 2015;86(5):1571–1587pmid:26280672
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    35. ↵
      1. Filmer D,
      2. Pritchett LH
      . Estimating wealth effects without expenditure data--or tears: an application to educational enrollments in states of India. Demography. 2001;38(1):115–132pmid:11227840
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    36. ↵
      1. Little RJA
      . A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(404):1198–1202
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    37. ↵
      1. Cohen J,
      2. Cohen P
      . Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1975
    38. ↵
      1. Martin A,
      2. Ryan RM,
      3. Brooks-Gunn J
      . When fathers’ supportiveness matters most: maternal and paternal parenting and children’s school readiness. J Fam Psychol. 2010;24(2):145–155pmid:20438190
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    39. ↵
      1. Malmberg LE,
      2. Lewis S,
      3. West A,
      4. Murray E,
      5. Sylva K,
      6. Stein A
      . The influence of mothers’ and fathers’ sensitivity in the first year of life on children’s cognitive outcomes at 18 and 36 months. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42(1):1–7pmid:26538379
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    40. ↵
      1. Brody GH,
      2. Dorsey S,
      3. Forehand R,
      4. Armistead L
      . Unique and protective contributions of parenting and classroom processes to the adjustment of African American children living in single-parent families. Child Dev. 2002;73(1):274–286pmid:14717257
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Linver MR,
      2. Brooks-Gunn J,
      3. Kohen DE
      . Family processes as pathways from income to young children’s development. Dev Psychol. 2002;38(5):719–734pmid:12220050
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Masten AS,
      2. Hubbard JJ,
      3. Gest SD,
      4. Tellegen A,
      5. Garmezy N,
      6. Ramirez M
      . Competence in the context of adversity: pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11(1):143–169pmid:10208360
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    41. ↵
      1. Burchinal M,
      2. Roberts JE,
      3. Zeisel SA,
      4. Hennon EA,
      5. Hooper S
      . Social risk and protective child, parenting, and child care factors in early elementary school years. Parent Sci Pract. 2006;6(1):79–113
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    42. ↵
      1. Makusha T,
      2. Richter L,
      3. Knight L,
      4. Heidi Van Rooyen P,
      5. Bhana D
      . The good and the bad?” Childhood experiences with fathers and their influence on women’s expectations and men’s experiences of fathering in rural Kwazulu-Natal, South Africa. Fathering. 2013;11(2):138–158
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    43. ↵
      1. Richter L,
      2. Chikovore J,
      3. Makusha T
      . The status of fatherhood and fathering in South Africa. Child Educ. 2010;86(6):360–365
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    • Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    PreviousNext
    Back to top

    Advertising Disclaimer »

    In this issue

    Pediatrics
    Vol. 138, Issue 4
    1 Oct 2016
    • Table of Contents
    • Index by author
    View this article with LENS
    PreviousNext
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Paternal Stimulation and Early Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Request Permissions
    Article Alerts
    Log in
    You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
    Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
    Citation Tools
    Paternal Stimulation and Early Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries
    Joshua Jeong, Dana Charles McCoy, Aisha K. Yousafzai, Carmel Salhi, Günther Fink
    Pediatrics Oct 2016, 138 (4) e20161357; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1357

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Share
    Paternal Stimulation and Early Child Development in Low- and Middle-Income Countries
    Joshua Jeong, Dana Charles McCoy, Aisha K. Yousafzai, Carmel Salhi, Günther Fink
    Pediatrics Oct 2016, 138 (4) e20161357; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-1357
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    Print
    Download PDF
    Insight Alerts
    • Table of Contents

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Conclusions
      • Acknowledgments
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & Data
    • Supplemental
    • Info & Metrics
    • Comments

    Related Articles

    • PubMed
    • Google Scholar

    Cited By...

    • Intimate Partner Violence, Maternal and Paternal Parenting, and Early Child Development
    • Is integrated private-clinic based early child development care effective? A clustered randomised trial in Pakistan
    • Time and Money: Extending Fathers Role in Economically Challenging Contexts
    • Google Scholar

    More in this TOC Section

    • Relational, Emotional, and Pragmatic Attributes of Ethics Consultations at a Children’s Hospital
    • Verbal Autopsies for Out-of-Hospital Infant Deaths in Zambia
    • Uncertainty at the Limits of Viability: A Qualitative Study of Antenatal Consultations
    Show more Article

    Similar Articles

    Subjects

    • International Child Health
      • International Child Health
    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders
    • Journal Info
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Policies
    • Overview
    • Licensing Information
    • Authors/Reviewers
    • Author Guidelines
    • Submit My Manuscript
    • Open Access
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Librarians
    • Institutional Subscriptions
    • Usage Stats
    • Support
    • Contact Us
    • Subscribe
    • Resources
    • Media Kit
    • About
    • International Access
    • Terms of Use
    • Privacy Statement
    • FAQ
    • AAP.org
    • shopAAP
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
    • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
    • RSS
    American Academy of Pediatrics

    © 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics