Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Eating in the Absence of Hunger and Weight Gain in Low-income Toddlers

Katharine Asta, Alison L. Miller, Lauren Retzloff, Katherine Rosenblum, Niko A. Kaciroti and Julie C. Lumeng
Pediatrics May 2016, 137 (5) e20153786; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-3786
Katharine Asta
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alison L. Miller
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
bDepartments of Health Behavior and Health Education,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lauren Retzloff
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katherine Rosenblum
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
cDepartments of Psychiatry, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Niko A. Kaciroti
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
dBiostatistics, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Julie C. Lumeng
aCenter for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
eNutritional Sciences, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, Michigan; and
fPediatrics and Communicable Diseases, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, Michigan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: To identify predictors of eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) in low-income toddlers, describe affect during EAH, test EAH as a predictor of body mass index (BMI), and examine the type of food eaten as a predictor of BMI.

METHODS: EAH, indexed as kilocalories (sweet, salty, and total) of palatable foods consumed after a satiating meal, was measured (n = 209) at ages 21, 27, and 33 months. Child gender, age, race/ethnicity, and previous exposure to the foods; maternal education and depressive symptoms; and family chaos, food insecurity, and structure were obtained via questionnaire. Child and mother BMI were measured. Child affect was coded from videotape. Linear regression was used to examine predictors of EAH and the association of kilocalories consumed and affect with 33 month BMI z-score (BMIz).

RESULTS: Predictors of greater total kilocalories included the child being a boy (P < .01), being older (P < .001), and greater maternal education (P < .01). Being in the the top quartile of sweet kilocalories consumed at 27 months and showing negative affect at food removal had higher BMIz (β = 0.29 [95% confidence interval 0.10 to 0.48] and β = 0.34 [95% confidence interval, 0.12 to 0.56], respectively).There was no association of salty kilocalories consumed or positive affect with BMIz.

CONCLUSIONS: There was little evidence that maternal or family characteristics contribute to EAH. EAH for sweet food predicts higher BMIz in toddlerhood. Studies investigating the etiology of EAH and interventions to reduce EAH in early childhood are needed.

  • Abbreviations:
    BMIz —
    BMI z score
    CHAOS —
    Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale
    EAH —
    eating in the absence of hunger
    WLZ —
    weight-for-length z score
  • What’s Known on This Subject:

    Eating in the absence of hunger (EAH) is a measurable, genetically linked behavior that is associated with overweight in children ages 3 to 13 years. Previous work has shown the potential for behavioral intervention.

    What This Study Adds:

    EAH occurs in toddlers. Consumption of sweet, but not salty, foods increases across toddlerhood during EAH, and greater EAH predicts greater adiposity at age 33 months.

    Eating behaviors, including greater food responsiveness, greater food enjoyment, less satiety responsiveness, lesser capacity to voluntarily inhibit eating, greater impulsivity, and lesser self-control have been linked with greater obesity risk and have been the topic of substantial investigation.1 The continued consumption of foods past satiety, referred to as eating in the absence of hunger (EAH),2 is correlated with greater food responsiveness and enjoyment, and less satiety responsiveness,3 as well as greater adiposity.4–15 Interventions that reduce responsiveness to food cues16–18 have been shown to reduce EAH, but interventions that increase children’s awareness of hunger and satiety cues had no effect.16 Thus, EAH may primarily reflect food enjoyment and responsiveness as opposed to sensitivity to hunger and satiety cues.

    There are a number of gaps in the existing literature. First, we have been unable to identify any studies of EAH that have focused on low-income children, who are at particularly high risk for childhood obesity19 or children younger than age 3 years. Second, though some studies have examined the stability or change in EAH with age,5,6,9–11,15 none have done so in children younger than 4 years. Third, studies examining maternal and family characteristics as predictors of child EAH2,4,5,7–11, 14,20–23 have been in predominantly white cohorts with the exception of 1 study that examined only Hispanic children.8 Fourth, children exhibit a liking for sweet taste even in the newborn period,24,25 but whether the association between EAH and adiposity differs based on the type of food eaten in the absence of hunger has not been described. In addition, most studies on early childhood2,4–15,20–22, 26,27 examine only the amount consumed as an outcome; we have been unable to find any studies that have also examined children’s affect during the EAH protocol and how this affect may be associated with child adiposity. We posit that affect during the EAH protocol reflects food enjoyment and responsiveness, each of which have been correlated with kilocalories consumed during EAH3 and greater adiposity in older children.28

    Therefore, within a diverse cohort of low-income children followed longitudinally at ages 21, 27, and 33 months, we sought to address 4 objectives: (1) to examine child, maternal, and family characteristics predictive of EAH across toddlerhood; (2) to describe child affect during EAH; (3) to test the prospective association of EAH with BMI z-score (BMIz) at age 33 months; and (4) to examine the type of food eaten and the association with BMIz.

    Methods

    Participants and Recruitment

    Participants were recruited via flyers posted in community agencies serving low-income families between 2011 and 2014. The study was described as examining whether children with different levels of stress eat differently. Inclusion criteria were that the biological mother was the legal guardian, had an education level less than a 4-year college degree, and was at least 18 years old; the family was eligible for Head Start, the Women Infant and Children Program, or Medicaid and was English-speaking; and the child was between 21 and 27 months old, was born at a gestational age ≥36 weeks, and had no food allergies or significant health problems, perinatal or neonatal complications, or developmental delays. Mothers provided written informed consent. The University of Michigan institutional review board approved the study.

    Mother–child dyads were invited to participate in 3 data collections at ages 21, 27, and 33 months; the data collection procedures at each age spanned across 5 days and included measures regarding eating behavior and biobehavioral self-regulation. A total of 244 dyads participated. Most (n = 186) dyads entered the study when the child was age 21 months, but 58 entered the study when the child was age 27 months to maximize recruitment; measures obtained at study entry are henceforth referred to as “baseline” measures. This report is limited to children who participated in at least 1 EAH protocol (scheduled to be obtained on the fifth day of data collection at each age) and one anthropometric measurement (scheduled to be obtained on the first day of data collection at each age). A total of 209 of the 244 participants participated in the EAH protocol at a minimum of 1 age point at which they also provided anthropometry. The 209 participants included in this analysis did not differ from the excluded participants with regard to child gender, child age, maternal BMI, maternal education, maternal depression, family chaos, food security, or family structure. Of the children with complete data included in this report, 47.4% were non-Hispanic white compared with 25.7% of those not included (P = .02). A total of 52 children (24.9%) participated at only 1 age point, 85 (40.7%) participated at only 2 age points, and 72 (34.4%) participated at all 3 age points. Mother–child dyads who participated at 2 or 3 age points did not differ at baseline from those who participated at only 1 age point with regard to child gender, child age, child race/ethnicity, maternal BMI, maternal education, maternal depressive symptoms, food security, or family structure. Those in the sample who participated at 2 or 3 age points, compared with those who participated at only 1, were more likely to report higher family chaos (Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale [CHAOS] score, 4.3 [SD, 3.3] vs 3.1 [SD, 3.0] [P = .03]).

    EAH Protocol

    Mother–child dyads were invited to participate in a standardized protocol2 to assess the child’s EAH at ages 21, 27, and 33 months in the child’s home. The mother was asked to have the child fast for 1 hour and then serve the child a typical lunch that included at least 2 different foods and 1 drink.

    After the lunch ended, a research assistant presented a standardized plate of foods (Table 1) and told the child, “Here are some special treats you can eat.” Mothers reported how often the child had eaten the food in the past 4 weeks (Table 1). To reduce food neophobia,29 the experimenter ate 1 Oreo cookie off of the plate and said, “I'm going to have one, too. Mmm, this is really good. You can eat as much as you want.” The child was given free access to the food. The mother was asked not to interact with the child during the protocol. After 10 minutes, the plate of food was removed. The remaining food was weighed and the amount consumed was calculated.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 1

    Foods Presented in EAH

    Affect was coded from video on a scale from 0 (none) to 2 (high intensity). Positive affect was coded for the 10 seconds during which the research assistant was delivering the plate to the child and the 40 seconds after plate presentation. Negative affect was coded for the 10 seconds during which the research assistant was moving to remove the plate from the child and the 10 seconds after plate removal. Inter-rater reliability was high (Cohen’s κ > 0.80). A child whose positive affect was >0 after plate presentation was categorized as having “positive affect at food presentation.” A child whose negative affect was >0 after plate removal was categorized as having “negative affect at food removal.”

    Anthropometry

    Weight, length, and height of the child were measured by trained research staff. Weight-for-length z-score (WLZ) and BMIz were calculated based on the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts.30 Mothers’ weight and height were measured and BMI calculated.

    Questionnaires

    Mothers reported child gender, birth date, and race and ethnicity; for this analysis, child race/ethnicity was categorized as non-Hispanic white versus not. Mothers reported maternal education (more than a high school diploma versus not), and family structure (single mother versus not). The Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression scale is a valid, reliable 20-item questionnaire31 designed to measure depressive symptoms. Mothers respond to a scale ranging from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or all of the time), and responses are summed so that a higher score indicates more symptoms (range, 0–60). CHAOS32 consists of 15 statements (true = 1, false = 0) that are designed to assess chaos in the home environment; scores range from 0 to 15, with higher scores reflecting more chaos. The US Department of Agriculture 18-item Household Food Security Survey33 categorizes households as food secure versus not.

    Statistical Analysis

    Analyses were conducted by using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). Univariate statistics were used to describe the sample. Bivariate analyses were conducted by using t tests and χ2 tests. One-way repeated measure ANOVAs were used to test whether total, sweet, and salty calorie consumption differed across 21, 27, and 33 months. Cochran Q tests were used to test whether positive affect at food presentation and negative affect at food removal differed across 21, 27, and 33 months. If ANOVA or Cochran Q test results were significant (P < .05), then post hoc comparisons were conducted using paired t tests or McNemar tests.

    Generalized estimating equations, using all available cases, were used to predict calories consumed, positive affect at food presentation, and negative affect at food removal, accounting for repeated measures within subjects. Predictor variables were added in 3 steps: child characteristics (gender, age, race/ethnicity, and previous exposure to the EAH foods), maternal characteristics (BMI, education, and depressive symptoms); and family characteristics (household chaos, food security, and family structure). Child age, maternal BMI, depressive symptoms, and family structure were time varying covariates. At each step, variables that were statistically significant (P < .05) were retained and 95% confidence intervals were calculated. For binary variables, parameter estimates were converted to odds ratios.

    Five multivariate linear regression models were used to predict child BMIz at 33 months from kilocalories consumed (total, sweet, and salty), positive affect at food presentation, and negative affect at food removal. To ease interpretation and given preliminary models suggesting that the relationship was nonlinear, we categorized kilocalories consumed into quartiles and compared the top quartile to the bottom 3 quartiles. The top quartiles at age 21 months were ≥114.3 kcal total, ≥84.1 kcal sweet, and ≥40.8 kcal salty. The top quartiles at age 27 months were ≥147.7 kcal total, ≥122.5 kcal sweet, and ≥46.2 kcal salty. These 5 models were fitted for predictors measured at age 21 months and again for predictors measured at age 27 months. Each model was adjusted for child gender, child race/ethnicity, maternal education, maternal BMI, food insecurity, and baseline child WLZ. These models followed the maximum likelihood (ML) approach, which produces valid estimates when missing data are missing at random.

    Results

    Characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 2. The sample was 51.2% boys, 51.2% white, 25.8% black, and 11.0% Hispanic. The mean WLZ at study entry was 0.51 (SD 1.06). Among the mothers, 38.3% had an education level of a high school diploma or less. The sample size of participants contributing to analyses at each age is shown in Table 3. Amount of food consumed and affect during EAH are shown in Table 4 across ages. Total kilocalories consumed increased with age (P < .001). When examined separately by food type (sweet vs salty), there were only significant increases in the amount of sweet food, but not salty food, consumed. Positive affect at food presentation also increased with age (P = .004). Negative affect at food removal did not change with age.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 2

    Characteristics of the Sample at Baseline (Study Entry at Either Age 21 or 27 mo; n = 209)

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 3

    Sample Size Contributing to Analysis at Each Age

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 4

    Food Consumed and Affect During EAH Across Age

    Table 5 shows results of the multivariate models testing predictors of EAH. Being a boy, older child age, and more maternal education each predicted more kilocalories consumed. Being a boy and older child age each predicted more kilocalories of sweet food consumed. Older child age and food insecurity predicted display of positive affect at plate presentation. None of the child, maternal, or family characteristics predicted kilocalories of salty food consumed or the display of negative affect at food removal.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 5

    Child, Maternal, and Family Characteristics Predicting EAH Kilocalories Consumed and Affect Displays (n = 209)

    Results of the multivariate models predicting BMIz at 33 months are shown in Table 6. Neither kilocalories consumed nor affect at 21 months predicted 33-month BMIz. Both kilocalories of total and sweet food consumed at 27 months predicted greater 33 month BMIz. Kilocalories of salty food consumed at 27 months was not associated with 33 month BMIz. Showing negative affect at food removal at 27 months was associated with higher 33 month BMIz. Positive affect at plate presentation at 27 months was not associated with 33 month BMIz.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 6

    EAH Food Consumed and Behavior by Age Predicting BMIz at 33 Months

    Discussion

    The main findings of this study were that EAH increased during toddlerhood, particularly for sweet foods. Greater intake of sweet food and the display of negative affect when the food was taken from the child predicted greater adiposity. To our knowledge, no other published reports have described EAH in children this young. The results of this study align with previous work in older children reporting that EAH increases with age5,6,9–11,15 and is associated with increased adiposity.4–15 The results also align with work in infants showing that parent-reported greater food responsiveness predicts greater prospective weight gain from ages 3 to 15 months.34

    Although older children showed positive affect at food presentation, this positive affect was not associated with greater BMIz at 33 months. Rather, negative affect at food removal predicted greater subsequent BMIz. To our knowledge, no other studies have observed the behavior of children in response to the EAH protocol, and this is the first report describing a link between affect during key points in the EAH protocol and weight gain.

    Boys were more likely to display EAH, especially for sweet food, which differs from previous literature that did not detect gender differences in EAH among older children.2,6–8,12–14,22, 26, 27,35,36 The only maternal characteristic related to EAH was maternal education. Maternal BMI was not associated with EAH, which aligns with findings of some studies,22 but not others.8,10,11 It was hypothesized that the characteristics related to household stressors in these low-income families (CHAOS, food insecurity, single mother family structure) would predict EAH. However, no association was found. EAH has previously been shown to be heritable via the fat mass and obesity-associated (FTO) gene.26,27 This behavior may be related to other biological factors that have yet to be identified.

    The association between EAH and future weight status did not emerge until age 27 months. EAH is believed to reflect a biological predisposition to increased food cue reactivity. The food cue reactivity is believed to be a type of Pavlovian conditioning.16 As such, it exemplifies a likely gene–environment interaction. For EAH and its effects on weight status to manifest, a child with a biological predisposition to increased food cue reactivity may need to have had sufficient exposure to food cues to elicit EAH. The findings suggest that the food cue reactivity is conditioned with repeated exposure to palatable foods before age 2 years, but the conditioned response is not detectable until age 27 months.

    Interpretation of these findings should note the study limitations. The home-based protocol reduced experimental control, but increased ecological validity. The presence of the researcher during the protocol may have influenced the child’s behavior. The fact that the researcher modeled eating a sweet food and not a salty food may have increased the likelihood that the toddlers ate sweet, as opposed to salty, foods. The longitudinal design is a strength, but because of the high-risk nature of the study cohort, attrition was high and there were missing data. Results may not be generalizable to other study populations outside low-income toddlers in the United States. Despite these limitations, the study was able to describe eating behavior in a very young age group longitudinally in a diverse population at a lower socioeconomic level than previous work.

    Conclusions

    Hedonic intake of sweet food is visible, starts to increase, and predicts weight gain in children younger than age 3 years. Given that EAH increases with age5,6,9–11,15 and behavioral intervention has been shown to reduce EAH,16 this study suggests that the timing of interventions targeting this behavior may need to occur before age 3 years. Developing interventions to reduce EAH that are developmentally appropriate for toddlers may be an important, novel intervention strategy. The lack of association with maternal and family characteristics suggests that further understanding of the etiology of EAH may require more studies about the underlying biology of this behavior.

    Footnotes

      • Accepted February 24, 2016.
    • Address Correspondence to Julie Lumeng, MD, Center for Human Growth and Development 300 North Ingalls St, 10th Floor, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. E-mail: jlumeng{at}umich.edu
    • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

    • FUNDING: This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 5R01HD069179. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

    • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. French SA,
      2. Epstein LH,
      3. Jeffery RW,
      4. Blundell JE,
      5. Wardle J
      . Eating behavior dimensions. Associations with energy intake and body weight. A review. Appetite. 2012;59(2):541–549pmid:22796186
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    2. ↵
      1. Fisher JO,
      2. Birch LL
      . Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite. 1999;32(3):405–419pmid:10336797
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    3. ↵
      1. Carnell S,
      2. Wardle J
      . Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite. 2007;48(1):104–113pmid:16962207
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. Birch LL,
      2. Fisher JO
      . Mothers’ child-feeding practices influence daughters’ eating and weight. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;71(5):1054–1061pmid:10799366
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    5. ↵
      1. Birch LL,
      2. Fisher JO,
      3. Davison KK
      . Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;78(2):215–220pmid:12885700
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    6. ↵
      1. Butte NF,
      2. Cai G,
      3. Cole SA, et al
      . Metabolic and behavioral predictors of weight gain in Hispanic children: the Viva la Familia Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(6):1478–1485pmid:17556682
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    7. ↵
      1. Cutting TM,
      2. Fisher JO,
      3. Grimm-Thomas K,
      4. Birch LL
      . Like mother, like daughter: familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers’ dietary disinhibition. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;69(4):608–613pmid:10197561
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    8. ↵
      1. Faith MS,
      2. Berkowitz RI,
      3. Stallings VA,
      4. Kerns J,
      5. Storey M,
      6. Stunkard AJ
      . Eating in the absence of hunger: a genetic marker for childhood obesity in prepubertal boys? Obesity (Silver Spring). 2006;14(1):131–138pmid:16493131
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    9. ↵
      1. Fisher JO,
      2. Birch LL
      . Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76(1):226–231pmid:12081839
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    10. ↵
      1. Francis LA,
      2. Birch LL
      . Maternal weight status modulates the effects of restriction on daughters’ eating and weight. Int J Obes. 2005;29(8):942–949pmid:15782227
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    11. ↵
      1. Francis LA,
      2. Ventura AK,
      3. Marini M,
      4. Birch LL
      . Parent overweight predicts daughters’ increase in BMI and disinhibited overeating from 5 to 13 years. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007;15(6):1544–1553pmid:17557992
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    12. ↵
      1. Hill C,
      2. Llewellyn CH,
      3. Saxton J, et al
      . Adiposity and ‘eating in the absence of hunger’ in children. Int J Obes. 2008;32(10):1499–1505pmid:18645573
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Kral TV,
      2. Allison DB,
      3. Birch LL,
      4. Stallings VA,
      5. Moore RH,
      6. Faith MS
      . Caloric compensation and eating in the absence of hunger in 5- to 12-y-old weight-discordant siblings. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(3):574–583pmid:22854400
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    13. ↵
      1. Moens E,
      2. Braet C
      . Predictors of disinhibited eating in children with and without overweight. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45(6):1357–1368pmid:17101117
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    14. ↵
      1. Shunk JA,
      2. Birch LL
      . Girls at risk for overweight at age 5 are at risk for dietary restraint, disinhibited overeating, weight concerns, and greater weight gain from 5 to 9 years. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(7):1120–1126pmid:15215771
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    15. ↵
      1. Boutelle KN,
      2. Zucker NL,
      3. Peterson CB,
      4. Rydell SA,
      5. Cafri G,
      6. Harnack L
      . Two novel treatments to reduce overeating in overweight children: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79(6):759–771pmid:22122291
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Boutelle KN,
      2. Zucker N,
      3. Peterson CB,
      4. Rydell S,
      5. Carlson J,
      6. Harnack LJ
      . An intervention based on Schachter’s externality theory for overweight children: the regulation of cues pilot. J Pediatr Psychol. 2014;39(4):405–417pmid:24459240
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    16. ↵
      1. Boutelle KN,
      2. Kuckertz JM,
      3. Carlson J,
      4. Amir N
      . A pilot study evaluating a one-session attention modification training to decrease overeating in obese children. Appetite. 2014;76:180–185pmid:24512975
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    17. ↵
      1. Shrewsbury V,
      2. Wardle J
      . Socioeconomic status and adiposity in childhood: a systematic review of cross-sectional studies 1990-2005. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008;16(2):275–284pmid:18239633
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    18. ↵
      1. Blissett J,
      2. Haycraft E,
      3. Farrow C
      . Inducing preschool children’s emotional eating: relations with parental feeding practices. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92(2):359–365pmid:20534744
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Fisher JO,
      2. Birch LL
      . Parents’ restrictive feeding practices are associated with young girls’ negative self-evaluation of eating. J Am Diet Assoc. 2000;100(11):1341–1346pmid:11103656
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    19. ↵
      1. Wardle J,
      2. Guthrie C,
      3. Sanderson S,
      4. Birch L,
      5. Plomin R
      . Food and activity preferences in children of lean and obese parents. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2001;25(7):971–977pmid:11443494
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    20. ↵
      1. Rollins BY,
      2. Loken E,
      3. Savage JS,
      4. Birch LL
      . Effects of restriction on children’s intake differ by child temperament, food reinforcement, and parent’s chronic use of restriction. Appetite. 2014;73:31–39pmid:24511616
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Blossfeld I,
      2. Collins A,
      3. Boland S,
      4. Baixauli R,
      5. Kiely M,
      6. Delahunty C
      . Relationships between acceptance of sour taste and fruit intakes in 18-month-old infants. Br J Nutr. 2007;98(5):1084–1091pmid:17521470
      OpenUrlPubMed
    22. ↵
      1. Mennella JA,
      2. Forestell CA,
      3. Morgan LK,
      4. Beauchamp GK
      . Early milk feeding influences taste acceptance and liking during infancy. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90(3):780S–788Spmid:19605570
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    23. ↵
      1. Fisher JO,
      2. Cai G,
      3. Jaramillo SJ,
      4. Cole SA,
      5. Comuzzie AG,
      6. Butte NF
      . Heritability of hyperphagic eating behavior and appetite-related hormones among Hispanic children. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007;15(6):1484–1495pmid:17557986
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    24. ↵
      1. Wardle J,
      2. Llewellyn C,
      3. Sanderson S,
      4. Plomin R
      . The FTO gene and measured food intake in children. Int J Obes. 2009;33(1):42–45pmid:18838977
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    25. ↵
      1. Carnell S,
      2. Wardle J
      . Appetite and adiposity in children: evidence for a behavioral susceptibility theory of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(1):22–29pmid:18614720
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    26. ↵
      1. Cashdan E
      . A sensitive period for learning about food. Hum Nat. 1994;5(3):279–291pmid:24214627
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    27. ↵
      1. Kuczmarski RJ,
      2. Ogden CL,
      3. Guo SS, et al
      . CDC growth charts for the United States: Methods and development. National Center for Health Statistics. 2002. Vital Health Stat 11. 2000;246:1–190
    28. ↵
      1. Radloff LS
      . The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–340
      OpenUrlAbstract
    29. ↵
      1. Matheny AP,
      2. Wachs TD,
      3. Ludwig JL,
      4. Phillips K
      . Bringing order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub and order Scale. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1995;16(3):429–444
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    30. ↵
      1. Bickel GNM,
      2. Price C,
      3. Hamilton W,
      4. Cook J
      . Measuring food security in the United States: Guide to Measuring Household Food Security. United States Department of Agriculture, Food, and Nutrition Service. Alexandria, VA: Office of Analysis, Nutrition, and Evaluation; 2000
    31. ↵
      1. van Jaarsveld CH,
      2. Llewellyn CH,
      3. Johnson L,
      4. Wardle J
      . Prospective associations between appetitive traits and weight gain in infancy. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94(6):1562–1567pmid:22071702
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    32. ↵
      1. Harris H,
      2. Mallan KM,
      3. Nambiar S,
      4. Daniels LA
      . The relationship between controlling feeding practices and boys’ and girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Eat Behav. 2014;15(4):519–522pmid:25090600
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    33. ↵
      1. Mallan KM,
      2. Nambiar S,
      3. Magarey AM,
      4. Daniels LA
      . Satiety responsiveness in toddlerhood predicts energy intake and weight status at four years of age. Appetite. 2014;74:79–85pmid:24316574
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    • Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    PreviousNext
    Back to top

    Advertising Disclaimer »

    In this issue

    Pediatrics
    Vol. 137, Issue 5
    1 May 2016
    • Table of Contents
    • Index by author
    View this article with LENS
    PreviousNext
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Eating in the Absence of Hunger and Weight Gain in Low-income Toddlers
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Request Permissions
    Article Alerts
    Log in
    You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
    Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
    Citation Tools
    Eating in the Absence of Hunger and Weight Gain in Low-income Toddlers
    Katharine Asta, Alison L. Miller, Lauren Retzloff, Katherine Rosenblum, Niko A. Kaciroti, Julie C. Lumeng
    Pediatrics May 2016, 137 (5) e20153786; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-3786

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Share
    Eating in the Absence of Hunger and Weight Gain in Low-income Toddlers
    Katharine Asta, Alison L. Miller, Lauren Retzloff, Katherine Rosenblum, Niko A. Kaciroti, Julie C. Lumeng
    Pediatrics May 2016, 137 (5) e20153786; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-3786
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    Print
    Download PDF
    Insight Alerts
    • Table of Contents

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Conclusions
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & Data
    • Info & Metrics
    • Comments

    Related Articles

    • No related articles found.
    • PubMed
    • Google Scholar

    Cited By...

    • Randomized Exposure to Food Advertisements and Eating in the Absence of Hunger Among Preschoolers
    • Google Scholar

    More in this TOC Section

    • Applications of Artificial Intelligence for Retinopathy of Prematurity Screening
    • Phenobarbital and Clonidine as Secondary Medications for Neonatal Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome
    • Severe Acute Neurologic Involvement in Children With Hemolytic-Uremic Syndrome
    Show more Article

    Similar Articles

    Subjects

    • Obesity
      • Obesity
    • Journal Info
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Policies
    • Overview
    • Licensing Information
    • Authors/Reviewers
    • Author Guidelines
    • Submit My Manuscript
    • Open Access
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Librarians
    • Institutional Subscriptions
    • Usage Stats
    • Support
    • Contact Us
    • Subscribe
    • Resources
    • Media Kit
    • About
    • International Access
    • Terms of Use
    • Privacy Statement
    • FAQ
    • AAP.org
    • shopAAP
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
    • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
    • RSS
    American Academy of Pediatrics

    © 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics