Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Review Article

Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review

Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, Candyce Hamel, Adrienne Stevens, Misty Pratt, David Moher, Suzanne P. Doucet, Deirdre Neuss, Anita Bernstein and Eunjung Na
Pediatrics January 2016, 137 (1) e20151974; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-1974
Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick
aFaculty of Health Sciences and
bChildren’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Candyce Hamel
cCentre for Practice-Changing Research, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Adrienne Stevens
cCentre for Practice-Changing Research, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
dTranslational Research in Biomedicine Graduate Program, University of Split School of Medicine, Split, Croatia;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Misty Pratt
cCentre for Practice-Changing Research, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Moher
cCentre for Practice-Changing Research, Ottawa Hospital Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
eSchool of Epidemiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Suzanne P. Doucet
fConsultant, Moncton, New Brunswick;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Deirdre Neuss
bChildren’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
gAudiology Clinic, Children’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anita Bernstein
hVoice for Hearing-Impaired Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eunjung Na
aFaculty of Health Sciences and
bChildren’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario Research Institute, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada;
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

CONTEXT: Permanent hearing loss affects 1 to 3 per 1000 children and interferes with typical communication development. Early detection through newborn hearing screening and hearing technology provide most children with the option of spoken language acquisition. However, no consensus exists on optimal interventions for spoken language development.

OBJECTIVE: To conduct a systematic review of the effectiveness of early sign and oral language intervention compared with oral language intervention only for children with permanent hearing loss.

DATA SOURCES: An a priori protocol was developed. Electronic databases (eg, Medline, Embase, CINAHL) from 1995 to June 2013 and gray literature sources were searched. Studies in English and French were included.

STUDY SELECTION: Two reviewers screened potentially relevant articles.

DATA EXTRACTION: Outcomes of interest were measures of auditory, vocabulary, language, and speech production skills. All data collection and risk of bias assessments were completed and then verified by a second person. Grades of Recommendation, Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) was used to judge the strength of evidence.

RESULTS: Eleven cohort studies metinclusion criteria, of which 8 included only children with severe to profound hearing loss with cochlear implants. Language development was the most frequently reported outcome. Other reported outcomes included speech and speech perception.

LIMITATIONS: Several measures and metrics were reported across studies, and descriptions of interventions were sometimes unclear.

CONCLUSIONS: Very limited, and hence insufficient, high-quality evidence exists to determine whethersign language in combination with oral language is more effective than oral language therapy alone. More research is needed to supplement the evidence base.

  • Abbreviations:
    ASL —
    American Sign Language
    GRADE —
    Grades of Recommendation, Assessment, Development, and Evaluation
    ITPA —
    Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities
    MD —
    mean difference
    PPVT —
    Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
  • Early detection of permanent childhood hearing loss through population-based newborn screening has become standard care in much of the world. Expectations are that early intervention through hearing technology will improve spoken language outcomes for children.1,2 Childhood hearing loss is a relatively frequent disorder, affecting 1 to 3 per 1000 live births,3–5 that disrupts typical language acquisition, placing children at risk for delays in language, literacy, and social development.6–8 There is strong consensus that specialized intervention must be combined with early identification to develop communication skills in these children.1,9

    Historically, considerable debate about optimal outcomes for children with hearing loss have resulted in the evolution of a plethora of intervention methods that constitute two broad but distinct philosophies. The oral approach aims to facilitate spoken language and inclusion with normal-hearing peers, and the manual approach focuses on visual communication systems (sign language) and a Deaf culture identity.10,11 Although there is wide recognition that various options should be available to families, 2 events in the past two decades (newborn hearing screening and cochlear implant technology) have made it possible for even children with profound deafness to develop spoken language.7,12–14 Epidemiologic data confirm that >90% of children with hearing impairment are born to parents with normal hearing.15

    Although there is substantial evidence that children with hearing loss can develop oral language skills,7,14,16 there is no consensus about optimal interventions.17 There is a common expectation that children receiving oral language intervention should develop better language skills than those who are also exposed to sign language, which might disrupt or delay spoken language acquisition. However, another body of research suggests that visual languages such as American Sign Language (ASL) are processed in the brain in the same manner as spoken languages and are complementary to auditory stimulation, and therefore provide a strong foundation for learning oral language.18,19 Therefore, adding sign language may develop bilingual skills and facilitate transition to spoken language acquisition. Anecdotal evidence supports various intervention options, but there is little scientifically based consensus. This information is essential to (1) guide families in making decisions about care for their children in infancy, (2) inform clinicians so that they can tailor treatment plans to achieve the desired outcomes, and (3) inform policy makers so that they can make optimal investments in early intervention services.

    This review is timely given the worldwide proliferation of screening programs in the past 10 years. In 2008, the US Preventive Services Task Force recommendations on newborn hearing screening highlighted the need for further research to demonstrate the effectiveness of the entire screening-to-intervention process.2 New possibilities due to screening and technology have reignited the discussion on best practices for children with hearing loss. Accordingly, the primary purpose of this research was to examine the evidence for the effects of various intervention options for early-identified children with hearing loss when the desired outcome is spoken communication. Our interest was in whether adding sign language facilitates spoken language, because of the increased focus on this outcome since the advent of cochlear implantation. Specifically, the review addressed the following question: Do children with hearing loss have better spoken language outcomes when exposed to early intervention that uses signs to support language compared with oral language intervention without sign language?

    Methods

    The protocol and report for this review were prepared according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses for Protocols,20 and the protocol was published21 and registered at PROSPERO (Registration #CRD42013005426),22 an international register of systematic review protocols. The review was undertaken using an integrated knowledge translation approach involving knowledge-user clinicians from health and education in the early stages and as required, to ensure relevance of the project for parents, clinicians, and decision-makers.

    Literature Search

    Ovid Medline In-Process & Other Non-Indexed Citations and Ovid Medline (1946 to June week 2, 2013), Embase (1974 to June 25, 2013), PsycINFO (1806 to June week 3 2013), and Cochrane CENTRAL (through May 2013 issue) databases were searched using the Ovid interface. CINAHL was searched on June 26, 2013, using the EbscoHost interface, and SpeechBITE was searched on June 26, 2013. The search strategy (Appendix 1) was developed in Medline by an experienced information specialist, peer-reviewed using the Peer Review of Electronic Strategies (PRESS) standard,23 and adapted for the other databases. The search was restricted to the pediatric age group,24 and the date was limited to material published in 1995 or later. A broad methodological filter was applied, incorporating published filters for controlled trials and other study designs.25,26 In addition, gray literature websites (A.G. Bell Association for the Deaf, National Acoustics Laboratory, National Health Services UK Newborn Hearing Screening Program, CADTH Gray Matters Checklist), conference proceedings (Newborn Hearing Screening Conference Abstracts 2010 and 2012), two journals (Ear and Hearing and Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education), and six books27–33 were searched.

    Eligibility Criteria

    Study Designs

    The following study designs were included: randomized controlled trials; controlled clinical trials and other quasi-experimental designs that include comparator groups; and prospective and retrospective cohort studies.

    Population

    Studies were eligible for inclusion if they included children (1) with early-onset (before age 3 years) hearing loss of any severity using hearing aids and/or cochlear implants; (2) receiving early intervention (age ≤3 years); and (3) <18 years old at assessment.

    Based on several studies that categorized participants with cutoffs at the 3-year age level, the early intervention criterion was adjusted from age 2 in our original protocol21 to age 3. We excluded studies that reported only outcomes for children with hearing loss who had developmental disabilities that interfered with spoken language.

    Intervention

    We included studies addressing early intervention aimed at spoken language development, which involved (1) oral language intervention and (2) any form of sign language (ASL) or sign support (eg, Signing Exact English, baby sign), commonly described under various names (eg, total communication, simultaneous communication). Relevancy of papers was assessed based on the components of the intervention described and not on the program label.

    The comparator of interest was oral language intervention without sign language.

    Outcomes

    Primary outcomes included all measures of spoken language including auditory, receptive, and expressive language skills (eg, vocabulary), speech production, and intelligibility. These outcomes were selected as clinically relevant based on a large body of literature.7,33,34 Secondary outcomes included electrophysiologic outcomes (eg, cortical responses). In addition, any adverse outcomes (eg, parent stress) were noted.

    Time Frame

    We included studies from 1995 onward to capture studies after wide implementation of cochlear implantation and newborn hearing screening. Earlier studies were excluded, as previous generations of children were unlikely to receive the same standards of early intervention and technology.

    Language

    Articles written in English and French were included.

    Study Selection

    All records were compiled in a Reference Manager database, checked for duplication, and exported to Distiller SR software35 for study selection, which involved 2 distinct stages. Screening forms were developed from inclusion and exclusion criteria and calibrated among reviewers with a subset of records before each screening stage. One reviewer assessed titles and abstracts for potential relevance; a second reviewer verified as not relevant all records coded as such. Two independent reviewers screened all potentially relevant full-text articles. Disagreements were resolved by consultation and consensus with a third researcher and knowledge-user clinicians as needed.

    Data Extraction

    A study-specific data extraction form, finalized with input from knowledge-user clinicians, was developed in Distiller SR to extract predetermined data variables. Items extracted included (1) study characteristics (citation, year, setting, country, language, publication status, and source of funding or other potential conflict of interest), (2) study design, (3) population characteristics (eg, sample size, gender, ethnicity, etiology, age, severity of hearing loss, hearing technology, and time with hearing technology), (4) details (type) of intervention, (5) details of comparison groups, and (7) outcome data. One researcher extracted data and a second researcher independently verified data. Discrepant and unclear data were resolved through consensus and included a third researcher if required. Outcomes reported only in graph format were estimated using Engauge software.

    We contacted study authors for clarifications related to (1) intervention age, (2) intervention method, and (3) metric (eg, standard score) reported for a test. Responses were received from all authors. No data were imputed for any outcomes.

    Study Quality Assessment

    Studies were assessed using the Qualitative Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies, developed by the Effective Public Health Practice Project at McMaster University to assess the quality of studies in a systematic review.36,37 The tool, accompanied by a reviewers’ dictionary, provides a methodological rating of studies based on an appraisal of 8 areas: selection bias, study design, confounders, blinding, data collection methods, withdrawals and dropouts, integrity of intervention, and study analysis. One researcher conducted the quality assessment after 2 studies were first assessed by 2 researchers, and the second verified the remaining assessments. A third researcher with content expertise rechecked all ratings and discussed any differences to reach consensus.

    Grading Strength of Evidence

    We applied methodology developed by the Grades of Recommendation, Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) working group38 to rate the quality of the overall body of evidence for each outcome. Primary outcomes were assessed across the domains of risk of bias, consistency, directness, precision, and publication bias. GRADE results in a rating of the quality of the body of evidence as high (very confident that true effect is close to the effect estimate), moderate (moderately confident), low (limited confidence), or very low (little confidence).

    Data Synthesis

    We summarized the characteristics of study populations and interventions in tabular and narrative form. A meta-analysis was not possible due to heterogeneity in designs, methods, and outcome metrics. A narrative synthesis and table summary of data were therefore completed. We present continuous outcomes as mean differences (MDs) with 95% confidence intervals, where possible. Planned subgroup analyses to examine variables related to hearing loss severity, age of identification, and hearing technologies could not be conducted owing to the lack of studies.

    Results

    We identified 432 records through database searching and 113 records through other sources, of which 470 were retained for screening after duplicates were removed. Figure 1 outlines the flow of records through the screening process. We assessed 352 full-text documents. As shown, the primary reasons for exclusion were (1) not a study design (58%) and (2) not an intervention of interest (32%). Eleven unique studies were included.

    FIGURE 1
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 1

    Study selection.

    Study Characteristics

    Table 1 summarizes the study characteristics. All were prospective cohort studies published between 1999 and 2013, 4 since 2004. The majority (n = 8) were conducted in the United States,32,39–45 and one each was conducted in Spain,46 the UK,47 and Denmark.48 Three studies44,46,47 were conducted specifically to examine whether signs added to spoken language intervention (sign + oral) improved outcomes. The remaining studies evaluated hearing technology or provided data about language intervention as 1 of several predictor variables in the analysis.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 1

    Characteristics of Included Studies

    Participants and Interventions

    Study size for reported outcomes ranged from 13 to 90 participants. It is not possible to rule out overlap of participants in some studies; for example, 339,42,43 from 1999 to 2002 included a subset of children from 1 US cochlear implant program. Eight studies included only children with severe to profound deafness who had received cochlear implants. The 3 remaining studies included a mix of children with respect to technology use.32,44,47 As shown in Table 1, authors used various descriptions and terms to describe both the intervention (eg, sign, bilingual) and comparator (oral, spoken language) groups.

    Quality Assessment

    Study quality and scores are shown in Table 1. Studies were rated as moderate (n = 4) or weak (n = 7) quality. Applying the McMaster Quality Assessment ratings,37 an overall weak rating is assigned if ≥2 areas are rated as weak. It is important to note that in these studies it was not possible to blind the examiners to the intervention, as children’s communication mode is generally reflective of intervention method; therefore, lack of blinding resulted in a weak rating as per the tool’s criteria. Other primary reasons contributing to an overall weak quality rating included limited information on confounders or data collection methods, which involved sign language adaptation of standardized test administration such that responses may not entirely reflect spoken language abilities. As noted, most included studies were not specifically designed to answer the review question. Consequently, the different focus for these studies may have influenced their quality ratings; that is, studies may have included fewer details about intervention methods, as it was not their primary question.

    Outcomes

    For reporting purposes, outcomes have been categorized into key domains (eg, language, speech) commonly used in auditory-language therapy (Table 2). Most studies (n = 10) reported multiple outcomes of interest, whereas 1 study41 reported speech perception only (Table 3). As shown in Table 2, different metrics (eg, standard score, raw score, age equivalency, language quotient) were reported in different studies. Studies also reported variations in test administration. In 1 study, Spanish test versions (which authors reported as validated) were administered.46 Danish test adaptations48 were used with American norms in another study except for the expressive vocabulary test, which was developed and standardized in Danish. As noted in Table 1, tests were administered in the child’s preferred modality (sign or spoken language) in 4 studies39,40,42,43 or with adaptations (for 1 test) in 1 study,32 and responses were accepted in sign and/or spoken language in another.45 These adjustments in test procedures can potentially affect the psychometric properties of the tests, which were standardized on children with normal hearing, and hence interpretation of the results. No study reported any adverse effects of the intervention. No electrophysiologic (secondary) outcomes were reported.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 2

    Summary of Evidence and GRADE Ratings

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 3

    Overview of Outcome Measures for All Included Studies

    Language Results: Vocabulary

    Receptive vocabulary was reported in 4 studies using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT), a norm-referenced test for ages 2.6 to 90+ years.49 All 4 studies included only children with cochlear implants.39,42,46,48 The 2 US studies39,42 were conducted in 1999 to 2000 and therefore included children implanted in the early years of cochlear implant availability. These studies reported no significant effect of intervention (communication mode). The third study46 reported standard scores from a Spanish version of the test, with no statistical difference between groups. One additional Danish study48 used a translated version of the PPVT with American norms. In that study, authors did not report scores separately by group but concluded that not using total communication was 1 factor associated with greater odds of age-equivalent receptive vocabulary.

    Three studies also reported results for expressive vocabulary. The Danish study48 examined expressive vocabulary using a Danish norm–referenced test but found no effect of intervention mode. The US preschool study32 found that expressive vocabulary results favored oral intervention only for late-identified children (>12 months) and showed no significant effects of intervention for early-identified children (<6 months). The late-identified group consisted of 12 children exposed to sign and 26 with no sign exposure (oral). One US cochlear implant study,45 whose primary goal was to investigate reading comprehension, also reported pre- and postimplant expressive vocabulary scores. The authors reported that sign + oral intervention was significantly associated with better vocabulary outcomes preimplant. However, as noted in Table 1, children in the study responded using sign and/or spoken words; therefore, this finding may not represent a measurement of spoken vocabulary. Postimplant vocabulary standard scores (74.60 vs 67.12) for both groups were well below the average for normal-hearing peers but slightly favored the sign + oral group. However, the authors did not state whether the difference was statistically significant.

    Language Results: Receptive and Expressive Language

    The most commonly reported outcomes for receptive and expressive language (5 studies) were from the Reynell Developmental Language Scales, a norm-referenced test for children ages 1 to 6 years. Four studies39,40,42,43 reported results for US children who received cochlear implants before 2002. According to the authors, there were no significant effects of intervention compared with controls (3 studies reported language quotients, and 1 study, language age scores). Only the Danish study48 using a Danish adaptation with American norms of the Reynell receptive subtest reported spoken language intervention to be statistically significant, for language comprehension only.

    The same 4 US cochlear implant studies also reported similar findings for expressive language measured by the Reynell test. The Danish study did not report expressive language. Only 1 study39 that favored spoken language intervention reached statistical significance.

    Three additional studies32,46,47 provided results for a variety of other receptive and expressive language measures, all yielding different results. As shown in Table 1, a US preschool study32 consistently reported no significant effects of intervention for early-identified children at 48 months. However, the study produced mixed findings for late-identified children, with results favoring oral intervention for 2 standardized measures (expressive language) and 1 language comprehension measure but no significant difference in interventions on another language comprehension test.

    Various subtest scores were also reported in the study of 18 children46 that used the Spanish version of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA). The authors reported significantly higher standard scores in auditory reception, auditory association, and grammatical closure for the spoken language–only group. Mean scores were significantly higher for the sign + oral group on the verbal expression subtest only. One additional study from the UK47 (n = 13) reported language outcomes using developmental scales to measure expressive language in the areas of pragmatics, semantics, and syntax. The authors reported delays ranging from 6 to 24 months in both groups and concluded that intervention did not influence outcomes.

    Language Results: Natural Language Sample Analysis

    Finally, 2 studies reported scores for various aspects of communication development. A study by Nicholas and Geers44 reported findings for 2 age groups, 18 to 30 months and 36 to 54 months, for 38 children who received sign + oral intervention and compared results to a historical cohort of 38 children in oral language intervention. As shown in Table 2, of 12 different results, statistically significant differences were reported only for words per utterance (sign or spoken words), which favored the sign + oral group at 18 to 30 months; number of different words spoken, which favored the oral group at 36 to 54 months; and the Proportion of Informative or Heuristic Functions, which favored sign + oral in both age groups. Of the 19 natural language results reported in the Nittrouer32 study, 18 showed no statistically significant difference for early-identified children (18 of 51 had sign + oral), whereas 1 measure (number of inquiries made by child) favored oral intervention. However, results were markedly different for the late-identified children (12 of 37 had sign + oral), in whom 14 of 19 results favored oral intervention.

    We retrieved only 1 study that addressed other aspects of language functioning considered relevant to this review. Using the Inventory for Client and Agency Planning, Jiménez et al46 reported no significant differences between groups in social communication skills.

    Speech Results

    Three studies addressed aspects of speech production, 2 related to speech intelligibility32,39 and 1 to phonological production.46 In 2 studies,39,46 scores were significantly better for the oral intervention groups based on author reports, whereas in the US preschool study,32 scores were significantly better for oral intervention in the late-identified but not early-identified children.

    Speech Perception

    Four cochlear implant studies39,41–43 (1999 to 2002) specifically investigated speech perception and included 3 closed-set measures (response selected from defined word set) and 2 open-set measures (no word set available). All scores were significantly better for the oral group, with the exception of 1 closed-set test (Mr. Potato Head, words) in 141 (n = 36) of the 4 studies, which was not statistically significant. However, for open-set monosyllabic words, the differences were clinically small despite statistical significance (eg, 20% vs 24%). No speech recognition measures were reported for children with hearing aids.

    Table 3 provides an overview of measures extracted in the included studies and the interventions that showed statistically significant differences. Of 61 total test scores reported, only 4 showed statistically significant results for sign + oral intervention and 36 for oral language only (9 related to speech perception, 17 for late-identified group in US preschool study32).

    Discussion

    There continue to be questions about the advantages or disadvantages of adding sign language to interventions focused on spoken language development in children with hearing loss. This review found that few studies have systematically addressed the issue. Eight of the 11 studies in this review included only children with severe to profound deafness who were using cochlear implants. This is likely because many children received preimplant intervention with a signing component in addition to spoken language, owing to the profound nature of hearing loss, particularly in the 1990s when cochlear implantation was a new option. Evidence to address the issue for children with less than profound degrees of hearing loss is severely lacking, as this review retrieved only 3 studies32,44,47 that included children with hearing loss using hearing aids.

    Measures of language development, including receptive vocabulary and receptive and expressive language skills, contributed the most information to this review. When comparing sign and oral language intervention with oral language intervention alone, individual studies showed no differences between groups for receptive and expressive vocabulary outcomes, with the exception of 1 study’s32 results that favored oral intervention only for late-identified children (n = 12 in sign group). However, a variety of measures and metrics were used across studies that limit the generalizability of the evidence. Overall, mixed results of statistical significance and no differences were observed in studies for receptive and expressive language outcomes. Three studies addressing speech significantly favored oral language therapy only (except 1 early-identified group32). Closed-set speech perception studies mostly favored spoken language only, whereas open-set speech perception studies statistically favored spoken language but with small clinical significance. The majority of studies were methodologically weak, with a few scoring a moderate rating. However, when rated according to GRADE criteria, the overall quality of the evidence per outcome is very low.

    According to GRADE, cohort studies are initially considered as low quality before applying the assessment criteria, as they are at a greater risk of bias, generally. The risk of bias for the body of evidence for outcomes was deemed to have serious limitations. The overall quality of evidence for each outcome we assessed was limited because of the small number of included studies, which resulted in low ratings for the precision of results and an inability to assess for consistency. There was also considerable variation in the measurements of outcomes, which made comparison across studies difficult. Furthermore, the full process of care from time of diagnosis is unknown, as is whether crossover interventions occurred. Therefore, additional studies in contemporary cohorts of children are needed to supplement the evidence base. Most studies were conducted before 2002 with children with severe to profound deafness and therefore may not reflect current practice, in which children with congenital deafness are likely to be identified and receive early intervention well before 1 year of age. Accordingly, new information about the effects of intervention in children across the spectrum of hearing loss severity has the potential to change the conclusions.

    Since completing our review, we are aware of 1 recent retrospective cohort study conducted in Australia with young children who used cochlear implants that compared a sign and oral language intervention group (n = 10) with 2 oral intervention groups (auditory-oral, n = 14, and auditory-verbal, n = 18).50 No significant differences in receptive vocabulary, auditory comprehension, or expressive language were found across the 3 groups after controlling for confounders such as age of hearing loss management and family involvement in the intervention program. These results are aligned with the overall findings of our review and would not change the conclusions.

    Limitations

    Despite a comprehensive search by an information specialist, it is possible that some studies were not included owing to the range of descriptions of intervention methods. During study selection, program descriptions were sometimes difficult to discern. For example, it was difficult to know whether interventions included the same level of emphasis on spoken language when sign language was added. Although we contacted authors for clarity of interventions and outcomes, we did not contact them for additional sample characteristics or statistical data. A further important limitation is that 8 of the 11 studies, although meeting inclusion criteria, were not designed to directly examine the central question of this review.

    Implications for Practice

    Although the question of intervention effectiveness and specifically the contribution of sign language combined with oral language is longstanding, there is strikingly limited new research to guide clinical or policy decision-making. The question of the contribution of sign language to the development of spoken language remains elusive, and there has been little growth in studies that include comparator groups since previous reviews.51,52 Despite strong conclusions based on expert opinion and intervention results without comparator groups, this review indicates that very limited, and hence insufficient, evidence exists to determine whether adding sign language to spoken language is more effective than spoken language intervention alone to foster oral language acquisition.

    Conclusions

    This review showed that there are important gaps in knowledge concerning the effectiveness of sign and oral language intervention, compared with oral language intervention only, for children with hearing loss when spoken language is the intended outcome. To date, there is no evidence that adding sign language facilitates spoken language acquisition. However, this review also found no conclusive evidence that adding sign language interferes with spoken language development. Overall, the literature related to intervention methods for children with hearing loss lacks properly designed cohort studies of today’s generation of children. It will be important to conduct more research to supplement this evidence base and to update this review as those studies become available. Given the current context of widespread neonatal screening, data can be collected prospectively from diagnosis, allowing for more accurate data regarding onset, severity, and changes in hearing loss, as well as specific characteristics and changes related to intervention, than was possible for study participants in this review. Interventions should be reported with specific detail, and if definitions can be developed and adopted on an international level, comparisons of similar programs would be more feasible. In addition, agreement on a common set of core outcomes and reporting metrics would be very useful in comparing future study results.

    There is insufficient information to guide parents about contemporary cohorts of children who have benefited from early detection and early access to hearing technology. The field would gain from carefully controlled prospective studies with today’s children.

    Appendix 1: MEDLINE Search Strategy

    MEDLINE

    1. exp Hearing Loss/

    2. (hearing adj (loss or impair$ or disorder*)).tw.

    3. deaf$.tw.

    4. (prelingual$ or pre-lingual$).tw.

    5. (sensorineural$ or sensori-neural$).tw.

    6. congenital.mp.

    7. or/1-6

    8. auditory verbal.tw.

    9. ((speech or auditory) adj2 feedback).tw.

    10. cued speech.tw.

    11. (listen* and (spoken or speak*)).tw.

    12. oral approach$.tw.

    13. aural.tw.

    14. Lipreading/

    15. (lipread$ or lip read$ or speechread$ or speech read$).tw.

    16. or/8-15

    17. Manual Communication/ or Sign Language/

    18. (sign$ language or sign$ english).tw.

    19. ASL.tw.

    20. visual language.tw.

    21. (baby sign or infant sign).tw.

    22. or/17-21

    23. Communication Methods, Total/

    24. total communication.tw.

    25. simultaneous communication.tw.

    26. (multilingual or multi-lingual).tw.

    27. (bicultural or bi-cultural).tw.

    28. (bilingual or bi-lingual).tw.

    29. bi bi.tw.

    30. or/23-29

    31. exp clinical trial/

    32. clinical trial.pt. or randomized.ti,ab. or placebo.ti,ab. or randomly.ti,ab. or trial.ti,ab. or groups.ti,ab.

    33. ((control$ or clinical or comparative$) adj2 (trial$ or stud$)).mp.

    34. exp Epidemiologic studies/ or Case-control studies/ or Retrospective studies/ or Cohort studies/ or Longitudinal studies/ or Cross-sectional studies/

    35. between group design$.mp.

    36. control group$.mp.

    37. (cohort stud$ or longitudinal).mp.

    38. (case adj2 (series or control$)).mp.

    39. ((consecutive or clinical) adj2 case$).tw.

    40. ((control$ or intervention or evaluation or comparative or effectiveness or evaluation or feasibility) adj3 (trial or studies or study or program or design)).tw.

    41. or/31-40

    42. 7 and ((16 and 22) or 30) and 41

    43. 42 and ((Infan* or newborn* or new-born* or perinat* or neonat* or baby or baby* or babies or toddler* or minors or minors* or boy or boys or boyfriend or boyhood or girl* or kid or kids or child or child* or children* or schoolchild* or schoolchild).mp. or school child.ti,ab. or school child*.ti,ab. or (adolescen* or juvenil* or youth* or teen* or under*age* or pubescen*).mp. or exp pediatrics/ or (pediatric* or paediatric* or peadiatric*).mp. or school.ti,ab. or school*.ti,ab. or (prematur* or preterm*).mp.)

    44. limit 43 to (“in data review” or in process or “pubmed not medline”)

    45. 42 and (child* or adolescent or infan*).mp.

    46. 44 or 45

    47. limit 46 to yr=”1985 -Current”

    Acknowledgments

    We thank Margaret Sampson, MLIS, PhD, AHIP (Children’s Hospital of Eastern Ontario), for developing the electronic search strategies and Janet Joyce, MLIS, for peer review of the MEDLINE search strategy. We also thank Pauline Quach for assistance with the quality assessment.

    Footnotes

      • Accepted October 7, 2015.
    • Address correspondence to Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology Program, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Ottawa, 451 Smyth Road, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1H 8L1. E-mail: elizabeth.fitzpatrick{at}uottawa.ca
    • This review is registered at PROSPERO: #CRD42013005426, http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.asp?ID=CRD42013005426#.VWeaU-vqc6I.

    • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

    • FUNDING: This systematic review was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Knowledge Synthesis Grant (FRN-124600). The funding agency was not involved in developing the protocol and was not involved in any aspect of the review (data collection, analysis, or interpretation) or publications. Dr Fitzpatrick’s work is also supported by a CIHR New Investigator and a Canadian Child Health Clinician Scientist Program award. Dr Moher holds a University of Ottawa Research Chair. All researchers are independent from the funding agencies.

    • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. American Academy of Pediatrics, Joint Committee on Infant Hearing
      . Year 2007 position statement: principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics. 2007;120(4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/120/4/e898pmid:17908777
      OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    2. ↵
      1. Nelson HD,
      2. Bougatsos C,
      3. Nygren P; 2001 US Preventive Services Task Force
      . Universal newborn hearing screening: systematic review to update the 2001 US Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation. Pediatrics. 2008;122(1). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/122/1/e266pmid:18595973
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    3. ↵
      1. Fortnum HM,
      2. Summerfield AQ,
      3. Marshall DH,
      4. Davis AC,
      5. Bamford JM
      . Prevalence of permanent childhood hearing impairment in the United Kingdom and implications for universal neonatal hearing screening: questionnaire based ascertainment study. BMJ. 2001;323(7312):536–540pmid:11546698
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Prieve BA,
      2. Stevens F
      . The New York State universal newborn hearing screening demonstration project: introduction and overview. Ear Hear. 2000;21(2):85–91pmid:10777016
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. Watkin PM,
      2. Baldwin M
      . Identifying deafness in early childhood: requirements after the newborn hearing screen. Arch Dis Child. 2011;96(1):62–66pmid:21047829
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    5. ↵
      1. Kennedy CR,
      2. McCann DC,
      3. Campbell MJ, et al
      . Language ability after early detection of permanent childhood hearing impairment. N Engl J Med. 2006;354(20):2131–2141pmid:16707750
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    6. ↵
      1. Ching TY,
      2. Dillon H,
      3. Marnane V, et al
      . Outcomes of early- and late-identified children at 3 years of age: findings from a prospective population-based study. Ear Hear. 2013;34(5):535–552pmid:23462376
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    7. ↵
      1. Wake M,
      2. Poulakis Z,
      3. Hughes EK,
      4. Carey-Sargeant C,
      5. Rickards FW
      . Hearing impairment: a population study of age at diagnosis, severity, and language outcomes at 7-8 years. Arch Dis Child. 2005;90(3):238–244pmid:15723906
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    8. ↵
      1. Moeller MP,
      2. Carr G,
      3. Seaver L,
      4. Stredler-Brown A,
      5. Holzinger D
      . Best practices in family-centered early intervention for children who are deaf or hard of hearing: an international consensus statement. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2013;18(4):429–445pmid:24030911
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    9. ↵
      1. Marschark M,
      2. Spencer PE
      . Spoken language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children: Historical and theoretical perspectives. In: Spencer PE, Marschark M, eds. Advances in the spoken language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006:3–21
    10. ↵
      1. Durieux-Smith A,
      2. Fitzpatrick EM
      . History of the management of hearing loss in children. In: Seewald R, Tharpe AM, eds. Comprehensive handbook of pediatric audiology. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing; 2011:617–629
    11. ↵
      1. Thoutenhoofd ED,
      2. Archbold SM,
      3. Gregory S,
      4. Lutman ME,
      5. Nikolopoulos TP,
      6. Sach TH
      . Paediatric cochlear implantation: Evaluating outcomes. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.; 2005
      1. Geers AE,
      2. Moog JS,
      3. Biedenstein J,
      4. Brenner C,
      5. Hayes H
      . Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2009;14(3):371–385pmid:19155289
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    12. ↵
      1. Fitzpatrick EM,
      2. Crawford L,
      3. Ni A,
      4. Durieux-Smith A
      . A descriptive analysis of language and speech skills in 4- to 5-yr-old children with hearing loss. Ear Hear. 2011;32(5):605–616pmid:21415757
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    13. ↵
      1. Mitchell R,
      2. Karchmer M
      . Chasing the mythical ten percent: parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Lang Stud. 2004;4(2):138–163
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    14. ↵
      1. Niparko JK,
      2. Tobey EA,
      3. Thal DJ, et al; CDaCI Investigative Team
      . Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA. 2010;303(15):1498–1506pmid:20407059
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    15. ↵
      1. Spencer PE,
      2. Marschark M
      . Evidence-based practice in educating deaf and hard-of-hearing students. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010
    16. ↵
      1. Petitto LA,
      2. Katerelos M,
      3. Levy BG,
      4. Gauna K,
      5. Tétreault K,
      6. Ferraro V
      . Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. J Child Lang. 2001;28(2):453–496pmid:11449947
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    17. ↵
      1. Petitto LA,
      2. Zatorre RJ,
      3. Gauna K,
      4. Nikelski EJ,
      5. Dostie D,
      6. Evans AC
      . Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97(25):13961–13966pmid:11106400
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    18. ↵
      1. Moher D,
      2. Liberati A,
      3. Tetzlaff J,
      4. Altman DG; PRISMA Group
      . Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ. 2009;339:b2535pmid:19622551
      OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    19. ↵
      1. Fitzpatrick EM,
      2. Stevens A,
      3. Garritty C,
      4. Moher D
      . The effects of sign language on spoken language acquisition in children with hearing loss: a systematic review protocol. Syst Rev. 2013;2:108pmid:24314335
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    20. ↵
      1. Fitzpatrick EM,
      2. Stevens A,
      3. Garritty C, et al
      . A systematic review of the effects of using sign language on spoken language acquisition in children with early-identified hearing loss. PROSPERO 2013:CRD42013005426 Available at: www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/display_record.asp?ID=CRD42013005426#.VWeaU-vqc6I. Accessed October 13, 2015
    21. ↵
      1. Mcgowan J,
      2. Sampson M,
      3. Lefebvre C
      . An evidence based checklist for the peer review of electronic search strategies (PRESS EBC). Evid Based Libr Inf Pract. 2010;5(1):149–154
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    22. ↵
      1. Leclercq E,
      2. Leeflang MMG,
      3. van Dalen EC,
      4. Kremer LC
      . Validation of search filters for identifying pediatric studies in PubMed. J Pediatr. 2013;162(3):629–634.e2pmid:23084708
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    23. ↵
      1. Glanville JM,
      2. Lefebvre C,
      3. Miles JNV,
      4. Camosso-Stefinovic J
      . How to identify randomized controlled trials in MEDLINE: ten years on. J Med Libr Assoc. 2006;94(2):130–136pmid:16636704
      OpenUrlPubMed
    24. ↵
      1. Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Network
      . Search filters. Available at: www.sign.ac.uk/methodology/filters.html. Accessed June 27, 2013
    25. ↵
      1. Spencer PE,
      2. Marschark M
      , eds. Advances in spoken language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006
      1. Brisk ME,
      2. Harrington MM
      . Literacy and bilingualism: a handbook for all teachers, 2nd ed. Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; 2007
      1. Schwartz RG
      . Handbook of child language disorders. New York: Psychology Press; 2009
      1. Clark EV
      , editor. The proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual child language research forum. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1998.
      1. Hughes E,
      2. Hughes M,
      3. Greenhill A
      , editors. BUCLD21: Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; 1997.
    26. ↵
      1. Nittrouer S
      . Early development of children with hearing loss. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing; 2010
    27. ↵
      1. Fitzpatrick E,
      2. Durieux-Smith A,
      3. Eriks-Brophy A,
      4. Olds J,
      5. Gaines R
      . The impact of newborn hearing screening on communication development. J Med Screen. 2007;14(3):123–131pmid:17925084
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    28. ↵
      1. Tobey EA,
      2. Thal D,
      3. Niparko JK,
      4. Eisenberg LS,
      5. Quittner AL,
      6. Wang NY; CDaCI Investigative Team
      . Influence of implantation age on school-age language performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Int J Audiol. 2013;52(4):219–229pmid:23448124
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    29. ↵
      1. Partners E
      . Distiller SR [computer program]. Ottawa, Canada: Evidence Partners; 2011.
    30. ↵
      1. Thomas BH,
      2. Ciliska D,
      3. Dobbins M,
      4. Micucci S
      . A process for systematically reviewing the literature: providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews Evid Based Nurs. 2004;1(3):176–184pmid:17163895
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    31. ↵
      1. National Collaborating Centre for Methods and Tools
      . Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies. Hamilton, ON: McMaster University; 2008. Available at: www.ephpp.ca/tools.html. Accessed October 13, 2015
    32. ↵
      1. Balshem H,
      2. Helfand M,
      3. Schünemann HJ, et al
      . GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(4):401–406pmid:21208779
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    33. ↵
      1. Miyamoto RT,
      2. Kirk KI,
      3. Svirsky MA,
      4. Sehgal ST
      . Communication skills in pediatric cochlear implant recipients. Acta Otolaryngol. 1999;119(2):219–224pmid:10320080
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    34. ↵
      1. Robbins AM,
      2. Bollard PM,
      3. Green J
      . Language development in children implanted with the CLARION cochlear implant. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl. 1999;177(Supplement):113–118pmid:10214814
      OpenUrlPubMed
    35. ↵
      1. Osberger MJ,
      2. Zimmerman-Phillips S,
      3. Barker M,
      4. Geier L
      . Clinical trial of the CLARION cochlear implant in children. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl. 1999;177:88–92pmid:10214809
      OpenUrlPubMed
    36. ↵
      1. Kirk KI,
      2. Miyamoto RT,
      3. Ying EA, et al
      . Cochlear implantation in young children: effects of age at implantation and communication mode. Volta Rev. 2000;102(4):127–144
      OpenUrl
    37. ↵
      1. Kirk KI,
      2. Miyamoto RT,
      3. Lento CL,
      4. Ying E,
      5. O’Neill T,
      6. Fears B
      . Effects of age at implantation in young children. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl. 2002;189:69–73pmid:12018353
      OpenUrlPubMed
    38. ↵
      1. Nicholas JG,
      2. Geers AE
      . Hearing status, language modality, and young children’s communicative and linguistic behavior. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2003;8(4):422–437pmid:15448073
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    39. ↵
      1. Connor CM,
      2. Zwolan TA
      . Examining multiple sources of influence on the reading comprehension skills of children who use cochlear implants. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2004;47(3):509–526pmid:15212565
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    40. ↵
      1. Jiménez MS,
      2. Pino MJ,
      3. Herruzo J
      . A comparative study of speech development between deaf children with cochlear implants who have been educated with spoken or spoken+sign language. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2009;73(1):109–114pmid:19046778
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    41. ↵
      1. Janjua F,
      2. Woll B,
      3. Kyle J
      . Effects of parental style of interaction on language development in very young severe and profound deaf children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2002;64(3):193–205pmid:12090947
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    42. ↵
      1. Percy-Smith L,
      2. Busch G,
      3. Sandahl M, et al
      . Language understanding and vocabulary of early cochlear implanted children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2013;77(2):184–188pmid:23141802
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    43. ↵
      1. Dunn LM,
      2. Dunn LM
      . Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—4. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2006
    44. ↵
      1. Yanbay E,
      2. Hickson L,
      3. Scarinci N,
      4. Constantinescu G,
      5. Dettman SJ
      . Language outcomes for children with cochlear implants enrolled in different communication programs. Cochlear Implants Int. 2014;15(3):121–135pmid:24867056
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    45. ↵
      1. Schachter HM,
      2. Clifford TJ,
      3. Fitzpatrick E, et al
      . Systematic review of interventions for hearing loss in children. Ottawa, Canada: Health Canada; 2002
    46. ↵
      1. Kumar S,
      2. Young G,
      3. James DGH
      . Communication outcomes of children with permanent hearing loss developing speaking and signing concurrently: a review. Int J Speech-Lang Pathol. 2009;11(2):135–146
      OpenUrlCrossRef
    • Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    PreviousNext
    Back to top

    Advertising Disclaimer »

    In this issue

    Pediatrics
    Vol. 137, Issue 1
    1 Jan 2016
    • Table of Contents
    • Index by author
    View this article with LENS
    PreviousNext
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Request Permissions
    Article Alerts
    Log in
    You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
    Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
    Citation Tools
    Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review
    Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, Candyce Hamel, Adrienne Stevens, Misty Pratt, David Moher, Suzanne P. Doucet, Deirdre Neuss, Anita Bernstein, Eunjung Na
    Pediatrics Jan 2016, 137 (1) e20151974; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1974

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Share
    Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review
    Elizabeth M. Fitzpatrick, Candyce Hamel, Adrienne Stevens, Misty Pratt, David Moher, Suzanne P. Doucet, Deirdre Neuss, Anita Bernstein, Eunjung Na
    Pediatrics Jan 2016, 137 (1) e20151974; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-1974
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    Print
    Download PDF
    Insight Alerts
    • Table of Contents

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Conclusions
      • Appendix 1: MEDLINE Search Strategy
      • Acknowledgments
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & Data
    • Info & Metrics
    • Comments

    Related Articles

    • No related articles found.
    • PubMed
    • Google Scholar

    Cited By...

    • Progress in Helping Children Who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing Achieve Their Potential
    • Early Sign Language Exposure and Cochlear Implantation Benefits
    • Opportunities and Shared Decision-Making to Help Children Who Are Deaf to Communicate
    • Google Scholar

    More in this TOC Section

    • Nonpharmacological Interventions for Pediatric Migraine: A Network Meta-analysis
    • Adolescent Risk Behavior Screening and Interventions in Hospital Settings: A Scoping Review
    • Grief and Bereavement in Fathers After the Death of a Child: A Systematic Review
    Show more Review Article

    Similar Articles

    Subjects

    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders
    • Children With Special Health Care Needs
      • Children With Special Health Care Needs
    • Journal Info
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Policies
    • Overview
    • Licensing Information
    • Authors/Reviewers
    • Author Guidelines
    • Submit My Manuscript
    • Open Access
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Librarians
    • Institutional Subscriptions
    • Usage Stats
    • Support
    • Contact Us
    • Subscribe
    • Resources
    • Media Kit
    • About
    • International Access
    • Terms of Use
    • Privacy Statement
    • FAQ
    • AAP.org
    • shopAAP
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
    • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
    • RSS
    American Academy of Pediatrics

    © 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics