Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Head Growth and Neurocognitive Outcomes

Charlotte M. Wright and Alan Emond
Pediatrics June 2015, 135 (6) e1393-e1398; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-3172
Charlotte M. Wright
aPeadiatric Epidemiology and Community Health Unit, School of Medicine, MVLS College, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alan Emond
bCentre for Child and Adolescent Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: There is a lack of evidence on the value of head circumference (HC) as a screening measure. We aimed to describe the incidence of head centile shifting and the relationship between extremes of head size and later neurodevelopmental problems in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.

METHODS: HC was measured routinely at 2, 9, and 18 or 24 months and by researchers at ages 4, 8, 12, and 18 months. IQ according to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children was measured in research clinics at age 8 for all. Neurocognitive disorders (NCDs) were identified from chart review.

RESULTS: There were 10 851 children with ≥2 head measurements. At each age, 2% to 3% of children had scores that were < −2 or >2 SDs below or above the mean, but for most children this was only found at 1 age. More than 15% of children showed centile shifts, but less than one-third of these were sustained at subsequent measurements. Only 0.5% showed a sustained shift beyond the normal range. Children with consistently small heads were up to 7 times more likely to have an NCD, but 85% of children with small heads had no NCDs, and 93% of children with NCDs had head SD scores within the normal range.

CONCLUSIONS: Centile shifts within the normal range occur commonly and seem mainly to reflect measurement error. This finding makes robust assessment of the head trajectory difficult and may result in many children being investigated unnecessarily. Extreme head size is neither specific nor sensitive for detecting NCDs, suggesting that routine measurement of HC is unhelpful.

What’s Known on This Subject:

Microcephaly is known to be associated with neurocognitive disorders and increasing head size with hydrocephalus. Head circumference is widely measured in childhood, but its practical value as a screening test is unclear.

What This Study Adds:

Measured head size is not a stable characteristic and centile shifts occur very commonly, mostly reflecting measurement error or regression to the mean. Even where head size was consistently extreme, it was not a good predictor of later developmental problems.

The measurement of head circumference (HC) is routinely undertaken as part of the assessment of neurodevelopmental concerns, and it is widely assumed to be a useful clinical tool. The American Academy of Pediatrics (http://brightfutures.aap.org) recommends HC measurement and plotting as part of the complete physical examination 8 times in the first 2 years. In contrast, in the UK, measurement of HC is recommended only after birth and at 8 weeks due to a lack of positive evidence for its usefulness thereafter.1

Quality research on the use of HC has been very limited until now. Various developmental conditions are associated with micro- and (rarely) macrocephaly, but a recent clinical review suggested that it is only more extreme microcephaly that is usefully predictive of NCD.2 One UK study examined children who were referred with HCs crossing below the second UK 1990 percentile. They found that 60% had no identifiable pathology,3 although this low rate of pathology at least partly reflected the tendency for all children to show a decline on the earlier UK 1990 charts.4

HC is also commonly measured as part of surveillance of healthy infants, and thus constitutes a form of screening. Probably the most important pathology potentially identifiable by screening HC is hydrocephalus. A Norwegian study5 showed the importance of enlarging head size in the diagnosis of hydrocephalus and suggested a prevalence of 7 per 10 000 births, but it is not known how large an increase is usually seen in these children or how many other children show increases without underlying pathology.

The UK adopted the World Health Organization (WHO) HC standard in 2009 along with the WHO standards for weight and height. Although the weight and length/height standards underwent a detailed risk analysis before adoption,6 the HC standard was published later7 and so was not appraised. Since then it has become evident that European infants’ heads are very large compared with the WHO standards,4,8 but these large discrepancies are not widely recognized and do not seem to have resulted in strikingly higher rates of referral, suggesting that HC is not in fact widely used to make clinical decisions.

We thus set out to seek new evidence for the usefulness of HC as a screening test. Data from a large UK prospective population-based cohort study provided the opportunity to describe the incidence of extreme head size and head centile shifting and to investigate how this related to later neurodevelopmental problems.

Methods

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing population-based study investigating the health and development of children and young people. In 1991–1992, pregnant women who were resident in the former Avon Health Authority in southwest England were enrolled, resulting in a cohort of 15 247 pregnancies and 14 701 children who were alive at 12 months.9 The study Web site (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac) contains details of all of the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and local research ethics committees.

All of the children in the cohort received routine child surveillance examinations by local health services, with HC measured routinely by health visitors (children's public health nurses) at 2, 9, and either 18 or 24 months, depending on the district of residence. These measurements were entered into the local child health computer and were subsequently retrieved for use by the study.

A subset of 1432 of these children (Children in Focus subsample) also had their heads measured by research staff at ages 4, 8, 12, and 18 months. The staff measured heads using a nonelastic plastic tape around the maximum diameter of the forehead and occiput, with the tape wrapped around the head firmly but not drawn tight. Staff were advised to take 3 measurements and to record the highest value.

From the age of 7 years children were called to annual research clinics where a range of measurements were undertaken. These included completion of a short version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III), test10 measured at age 8. A low IQ was defined as total IQ <70.

Educational records linkage to the Pupil Level Annual Schools Census data set for 2003/2004 were used to identify all children recorded as receiving extra classroom support with a Statement of Special Educational Needs (SSEN) by the age of 11 years. This is a formal process used in the United Kingdom to establish the educational needs of children with severe learning difficulty. All cohort members who had a medical assessment for SSEN were identified from community child health records, and those children in the cohort admitted with a relevant developmental disorder were identified from linked hospital admission statistics. A retrospective notes review for all of these children was then undertaken by a team of researchers led by an experienced developmental pediatrician (A.E.) to allocate neurodevelopmental diagnostic International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision, codes as follows: learning disability (classified as mild or moderate/severe); specific deficits of speech, learning, or motor function; or autism, epilepsy, hyperkinetic disorders, and conduct disorders. Further details of the process used to identify neurodevelopmental cases in ALSPAC can be found elsewhere.11

There were only 2 known cases of hydrocephalus in the cohort, and these cases could not be identified to researchers due to the ALSPAC confidentiality policy concerning rare conditions.

Analysis

All head measurements were converted into z scores compared with the WHO growth standard.12 For each measurement, outliers (>6 and < −6 z) were excluded as well as those measurements collected at ages outside the age limits (see Table 1). These z scores were then standardized by subtracting the population mean to yield internally standardized scores with a mean (SD) z score of 0 (1) at all ages. A summary infancy head z score was calculated for each child by using the average of the internally standardized SD scores at 8 weeks and 9 months plus the 18- or 24-month value where available.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1

Number, Age Range, and Mean Values for Routinely Collected HC Measurements From the Main Data Set at Different Ages

Extreme head size was defined as head size below the second (−2 z) or above the 98th (+2 z) centile. Head size shift was determined by calculating the change in z score between 2 and 9 months (early) and between 9 and 18 or 24 months (late). Obvious outliers with shifts between measures of <5 z and >5 z were excluded. A sustained shift was then defined as a shift of ≥1 z in 1 interval with no net recovery in the next interval. Statistical analysis was undertaken by using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 21 (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY).

Results

The number of measurements for the total cohort varied from 12 087 at age 6 to 8 weeks to 7077 at age 18 or 24 months; 9279 children had 2 to 3 head measurements, 5378 of whom had all 3 measurements. As described previously,4 the mean z scores compared with the WHO standards were already just above the 75th centile at age 6 to 8 weeks and increased to between the 75th and 91st centiles by age 9 months (Table 1). This mismatch to the WHO standard meant that <1% were below the second centile at any age but up to 18% were above the 98th centile. By using the internally standardized z scores, between 2% and 3% were outside either threshold at each age, but three-fourths were only outside this threshold at 1 age (Table2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 2

Proportion of All Children in the Main Data Set With Extreme Measurements or Shifts in Head Size

In the research subset (n = 1312), similar proportions were outside the extreme thresholds at each age. Slightly more children had consistently extreme measures, but two-thirds were still extreme on only 1 measure (Table 3).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 3

Proportion of Children in Research Subset With Extreme Measurements or Shifts in Head Size Using Internally Standardized z Scores

Centile shifts were very common in the main data set. By using the WHO standard, upward shifts were much more common than downward shifts between 2 and 9 months. By using the internally standardized z scores, similar proportions shifted up and down; >20% crossed >1 z upward or downward between 6 to 8 weeks and 9 months and ∼15% crossed >1 z upward or downward between 9 and 18 or 24 months. However, approximately three-fourths with shifts between 2 and 9 months showed at least some recovery in the period up to 18 or 24 months and <0.5% showed a sustained shift to outside either the 98th or second centile (Table 2). In the research subset, centile shifts were much less common but still only approximately half of these were sustained shifts. As in the main set, shifts to outside either the 98th or second centile were rare (Table3). By using the internally standardized z score (mean = 0.01; SD = 0.88) in the main data set, 2.6% had summary infancy head z scores outside either the 98th or second centile.

Neurodevelopmental problems in the cohort

All of the individual neurodevelopmental diagnoses were rare, but of all the children with 2 to 3 head measurements 4.5% had some sort of neurocognitive disorder (NCD; ie, ≥1 neurodevelopmental diagnosis, an SSEN by age 11, or a low WISC IQ). Children with low summary head z scores had significantly increased risks of any neurodevelopmental diagnosis, a statement of SSEN at age 11, and low WISC IQ, with the largest excess seen for children with SSEN. Children with high average head SD scores only had a significantly increased risk of any neurodevelopmental diagnosis or of low IQ but no increased risk of an SSEN (Table 4). However, 85% of children with small average head z scores and 91% of those with large average head z scores did not have an NCD, whereas 93% children with NCDs had head z scores within the normal range (Table 4).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 4

Neurodevelopmental Problems in Those in the Main Data Set With 2 to 3 Head Measurements and Relation to Extreme Head Size

For the smaller subset of children measured in the research clinic, the proportion overall with NCDs was similar, although the absolute numbers were small (Table 5). In this subset, a much stronger association between small head size and NCDs was seen but there was no trend to an association with large heads. Nonetheless, ∼90% of children with NCDs had normal-sized heads and between two-thirds and three-fourths of those with small heads had no abnormality. There was no relationship between neurologic abnormality or low IQ with centile shifts in either direction in either the main data set or in the research subset (data not shown).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 5

Prevalence of NCDs in the Research Subset by Head Size at Different Ages

Discussion

The main findings of this analysis are that although, in principle, head size should be a stable characteristic, in practice it is not, with instances of extreme head size usually occurring in isolation and centile shifts occurring commonly. Even when head size was consistently extreme, this finding was not a good predictor of later developmental problems.

The strengths of this study are that it used a large population-based cohort of healthy children that was generally representative of UK children at the time and HC measures were collected as part of routine health care and were combined with those collected in research clinics. The main limitation is that there were a large number of observers; although all had received training according to a standard protocol for measuring head circumference, no information was available on how compliant the clinicians were in following recognized good practice. However, the health service staff in the Bristol area were probably no better or worse than other clinicians undertaking child health surveillance, so the findings are of relevance to other real-life settings. Missing data were a potential limitation, as in all longitudinal studies, but the very large scale of the cohort meant that there were large numbers for most analyses, and a majority of children were represented in most of the analyses.

These routinely collected HC measurements showed great within-subject variability, suggesting a high degree of measurement error. Even in a research setting, two-thirds of children who were recorded with small heads once were not found to have small heads again, which suggests that there is a high degree of practically unavoidable measurement error. As a result, apparent shifts in head centiles were frequent, but most were not sustained, with a decrease back to a less extreme centile at the next measurement point.

Probably the most important pathology that is potentially identifiable by increasing HC is hydrocephalus. The Norwegian study5 described above found that of all children with expansive intracranial pathology, half had been identified on the basis of HC crossing more than “2 percentile curves,” 80% of whom had no other symptoms or signs. However, our data suggest that 12% to 18% of children had made a shift of this size, depending on the chart used, with 3% to 6% showing a sustained increase. By using this criterion, to identify the 6 cases per 10 000 children identifiable by screening, at least 1200 children would need to be remeasured and 300 would need more substantial investigation. This level of re-measuring and investigation will create much parental anxiety, unnecessary cranial ultrasounds and CT scans and referral to specialists, solely for reassurance that the child is within the wide range of normal.

Centile shifts in our data occurred most commonly within the normal range, probably reflecting normalization toward the average (regression to mean). Centile shifts beyond the normal range, in contrast, were much rarer and these children may be those who would most merit investigation. What is not known is how large of an increase in HC was usually seen in the Norwegian case series or how many had crossed above the upper centiles, so more research in this area is needed.

With extreme small head size it is easier to identify a clearly abnormal group, by setting a low threshold (−2 z) and rechecking low measurements for consistency. In this data set, only ∼1% were consistently below the internally standardized second centile and these children did have up to a sevenfold increased risk of NCDs when measured in a research setting. Nonetheless, the great majority of children with NCDs had heads within the normal range and most children with small heads did not go on to have an NCD. This finding is compatible with a large US study in an unselected population that found that half the children with very small heads (3 z below the internally standardized mean) had an IQ <70,13 but for those with heads between −2 and −3 z only 10% had learning difficulties compared with 3% of those with heads within the normal range.

It is of note that we found no association between centile shifts and NCDs. An earlier analysis of the ALSPAC 10% substudy14 and 1 other study15 found that head growth during infancy was associated with IQ, but the observed IQ variation in these studies was small and all of the children studied were within the normal range.

When a measure is applied widely to otherwise healthy individuals it constitutes screening and raises the question of how well measurement of HC meets the criteria of a good screening test.16 On the positive side, HC measurement is quick, cheap, and safe and the conditions of interest are important. Hydrocephalus meets the essential criteria of being treatable, having a latent stage before becoming fully manifest, and benefiting from early identification, but most forms of NCDs do not. However, the key problem is that HC measurement has a high false-positive rate for both possible hydrocephalus and NCDs and a very low sensitivity for NCDs.

Conclusions

When HC is measured, clinicians and parents can be reassured that the majority of children with small heads will have no underlying pathology and that the commonest explanation for an increase or decrease in head centile is measurement error. Given the great variability in HC measurement, the more often a child’s head is measured, the more often spurious extreme or shifting head size will be seen. The implication of this variability is that repeated measurement of HC in infancy is unhelpful and should be undertaken only when an early measurement is outside +2 SDs. The guidelines for the Bright Futures Health Supervision visits recommend a “complete physical exam” including HC measurement at each visit. We suggest an HC measurement in the first 2–5 days after birth, but not in the first 48 hours to allow time for resolution of molding during birth17 and once more before the age of 6 months. Otherwise, HC should not be routinely measured as part of a complete physical examination, and we suggest that the American Academy of Pediatrics and the Healthy Child Program in the United Kingdom should reconsider their recommendations in the light of this evidence.

Acknowledgments

We thank all of the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. We also thank the staff at the Royal College of Paediatrics and Child Health who undertook the initial literature search for this article.

Footnotes

    • Accepted March 9, 2015.
  • Address correspondence to Charlotte M. Wright, MD, PEACH Unit, QMH Tower, Yorkhill Hospitals, Glasgow G3 8SJ, UK. E-mail: charlotte.wright{at}glasgow.ac.uk
  • Dr Wright conceptualized and undertook this analysis and drafted the manuscript; Dr Emond led the retrospective notes review and planned the data linkage process, negotiated the release of the data, helped plan the analysis, and edited the manuscript; and both authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

  • The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC.

  • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

  • FUNDING: No external funding.

  • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: Dr Wright led the project at the Royal College of Paediatrics and Child Health to develop new UK growth charts including the World Health Organization head circumference standard, for which her institution received funding from the English Department of Health; she has received no other financial benefit from this role; and Dr Emond has indicated he has no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  1. ↵
    Hall DMB, Elliman D. Health for All Children. 4th ed. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2003
  2. ↵
    1. Woods CG,
    2. Parker A
    . Investigating microcephaly. Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(9):707–713pmid:23814088
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Baxter PS,
    2. Rigby AS,
    3. Rotsaert MHEPD,
    4. Wright I
    . Acquired microcephaly: causes, patterns, motor and IQ effects, and associated growth changes. Pediatrics. 2009;124(2):590–595pmid:19651581
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Wright CM,
    2. Inskip HM,
    3. Godfrey K,
    4. Williams AF,
    5. Ong KK
    . Monitoring head size and growth using the new UK-WHO growth standard. Arch Dis Child. 2011;96(4):386–388pmid:21285227
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Zahl SM,
    2. Wester K
    . Routine measurement of head circumference as a tool for detecting intracranial expansion in infants: what is the gain? A nationwide survey. Pediatrics. 2008;121(3):e416–e420pmid:18310161
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition. Application of WHO Growth Standards in the UK 2007. Department of Health, London, United Kingdom; 2008
  7. ↵
    WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. WHO Child Growth Standards: Head Circumference-for-Age, Arm Circumference-for-Age, Triceps Skinfold-for-Age and Subscapular Skinfold-for-Age: Methods and Development. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2007
  8. ↵
    1. Júlíusson PB,
    2. Roelants M,
    3. Hoppenbrouwers K,
    4. Hauspie R,
    5. Bjerknes R
    . Growth of Belgian and Norwegian children compared to the WHO growth standards: prevalence below -2 and above +2 SD and the effect of breastfeeding. Arch Dis Child. 2011;96(10):916–921pmid:19948662
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Boyd A,
    2. Golding J,
    3. Macleod J,
    4. et al
    . Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111–127pmid:22507743
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Wechsler D
    . Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991
  11. ↵
    1. Williams E,
    2. Thomas K,
    3. Sidebotham H,
    4. Emond A
    . Prevalence and characteristics of autistic spectrum disorders in the ALSPAC cohort. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50(9):672–677pmid:18754916
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/Height-for-Age, Weight-for-Age, Weight-for-Length, Weight-for-Height and Body Mass Index-for-Age: Methods and Development. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2006
  13. ↵
    1. Dolk H
    . The predictive value of microcephaly during the first year of life for mental retardation at seven years. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1991;33(11):974–983pmid:1743426
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Gale CR,
    2. O’Callaghan FJ,
    3. Godfrey KM,
    4. Law CM,
    5. Martyn CN
    . Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in children. Brain. 2004;127(Pt 2):321–329pmid:14645144
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Heinonen K,
    2. Räikkönen K,
    3. Pesonen AK,
    4. et al
    . Prenatal and postnatal growth and cognitive abilities at 56 months of age: a longitudinal study of infants born at term. Pediatrics. 2008;121(5):e1325–e1333pmid:18450875
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Wilson JJ,
    2. Jungner YG
    . Principles and Practice of Screening for Disease. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1968
  17. ↵
    1. Simič Klarić A,
    2. Tomić Rajić M,
    3. Tesari Crnković H
    . Timing of head circumference measurement in newborns. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2014;53(5):456–459pmid:24288388
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  • Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
PreviousNext
Back to top

Advertising Disclaimer »

In this issue

Pediatrics
Vol. 135, Issue 6
1 Jun 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
View this article with LENS
PreviousNext
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Head Growth and Neurocognitive Outcomes
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Request Permissions
Article Alerts
Log in
You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Head Growth and Neurocognitive Outcomes
Charlotte M. Wright, Alan Emond
Pediatrics Jun 2015, 135 (6) e1393-e1398; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-3172

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Head Growth and Neurocognitive Outcomes
Charlotte M. Wright, Alan Emond
Pediatrics Jun 2015, 135 (6) e1393-e1398; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-3172
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Print
Download PDF
Insight Alerts
  • Table of Contents

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusions
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Factors associated with head circumference and indices of cognitive development in early childhood
  • Extreme prematurity, growth and neurodevelopment at 8 years: a cohort study
  • Does measurement technique explain the mismatch between European head size and WHO charts?
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Comparison of Manual and Automated Sepsis Screening Tools in a Pediatric Emergency Department
  • Romantic Relationships in Transgender Adolescents: A Qualitative Study
  • Patterns and Predictors of Professional Interpreter Use in the Pediatric Emergency Department
Show more Article

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
    • Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders
  • Community Pediatrics
    • Community Pediatrics
    • Bright Futures
  • Journal Info
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Policies
  • Overview
  • Licensing Information
  • Authors/Reviewers
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submit My Manuscript
  • Open Access
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Librarians
  • Institutional Subscriptions
  • Usage Stats
  • Support
  • Contact Us
  • Subscribe
  • Resources
  • Media Kit
  • About
  • International Access
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • FAQ
  • AAP.org
  • shopAAP
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
  • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
  • RSS
American Academy of Pediatrics

© 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics