Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Environmental Risk Factors by Gender Associated With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Desiree Silva, Lyn Colvin, Erika Hagemann and Carol Bower
Pediatrics January 2014, 133 (1) e14-e22; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-1434
Desiree Silva
aTelethon Institute for Child Health Research, Centre for Child Health Research, and
bSchool of Paediatrics and Child Health, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lyn Colvin
aTelethon Institute for Child Health Research, Centre for Child Health Research, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Erika Hagemann
cFremantle Speech Pathology Services, Fremantle, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Carol Bower
aTelethon Institute for Child Health Research, Centre for Child Health Research, and
bSchool of Paediatrics and Child Health, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia; and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

BACKGROUND: Early environmental risk factors associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been increasingly suggested. Our study investigates the maternal, pregnancy, and newborn risk factors by gender for children prescribed stimulant medication for treatment of ADHD in Western Australia.

METHODS: This is a population-based, record linkage case–control study. The records of all non-Aboriginal children and adolescents born in Western Australia and aged <25 years who were diagnosed with ADHD and prescribed stimulant medication (cases = 12 991) were linked to the Midwives Notification System (MNS) to obtain maternal, pregnancy, and birth information. The control population of 30 071 children was randomly selected from the MNS.

RESULTS: Mothers of children with ADHD were significantly more likely to be younger, be single, have smoked in pregnancy, have labor induced, and experience threatened preterm labor, preeclampsia, urinary tract infection in pregnancy, or early term delivery irrespective of the gender of the child, compared with the control group. In the fully adjusted model, a novel finding was of a possible protective effect of oxytocin augmentation in girls. Low birth weight, postterm pregnancy, small for gestational age infant, fetal distress, and low Apgar scores were not identified as risk factors.

CONCLUSIONS: Smoking in pregnancy, maternal urinary tract infection, being induced, and experiencing threatened preterm labor increase the risk of ADHD, with little gender difference, although oxytocin augmentation of labor appears protective for girls. Early term deliveries marginally increased the risk of ADHD. Studies designed to disentangle possible mechanisms, confounders, or moderators of these risk factors are warranted.

  • ADHD
  • risk factors
  • gestational age
  • birth weight
  • smoking in pregnancy
  • Abbreviations:
    ADHD —
    attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
    AGA —
    appropriate for gestational age
    CI —
    confidence interval
    GA —
    gestational age
    LGA —
    large for gestational age
    MODDS —
    Monitoring of Drugs of Dependence System
    MNS —
    Midwives Notification System
    OR —
    odds ratio
    SEIFA —
    Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas
    SGA —
    small for gestational age
    SM —
    stimulant medication
    UTI —
    urinary tract infection
    WA —
    Western Australia
  • What’s Known on This Subject:

    Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common mental health condition diagnosed in childhood, is highly heritable, and more common in boys. Although studies have identified perinatal risk factors, no one has investigated perinatal risk factors separately in boys and girls.

    What This Study Adds:

    Contrary to other studies, low birth weight, postterm pregnancy, low Apgar scores, and fetal distress were not risk factors for ADHD irrespective of gender. Early term deliveries increased the risk of ADHD, and oxytocin augmentation in girls may be protective.

    Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most common mental health disorder in childhood, with a global prevalence of 5.2%.1 The key features of ADHD are inattention with or without hyperactivity and impulsivity, which can result in a lifelong disorder with significant social and economic burden.2 Strong inheritance of ADHD is not disputed,3 with mean heritability estimated at ∼79% and male predominance.4 However, environmental factors are important.5 Environmental influences may correlate with, interact with, or mediate genetic influences and the outcome of ADHD and its symptoms.5–10 For example, genetic effects may be conditional on exposure to certain environmental risks. A recent literature review that focused on the genetic and environmental causes of ADHD suggested that after genetic factors, the strongest evidence of environmental factors relating to ADHD were the rare adversities that include extreme prematurity, very low birth weight, fetal alcohol syndrome, and behaviors associated with early institutionalized deprivation.10 Others have suggested that the strong genetic risk for ADHD can be moderated or mediated by prenatal (eg, exposure to maternal stress, alcohol, nicotine, lead, preeclampsia, and maternal urinary tract infection [UTI]), perinatal (prematurity, low birth weight, anoxic–ischemic encephalopathy, meningitis, encephalitis), and postnatal (viral meningitis, encephalitis, cerebral trauma) factors.6,8,9,11,12 However, population studies have shown varying results, from no elevated risk or low risk for ADHD due to perinatal factors13,14 to an elevated risk associated with prematurity and postterm babies.15,16 Recently there has been a focus on early term births (37–38 weeks’ gestation), which may also increase the risk of ADHD.17 Being first born has shown mixed results, from no increased risk18 to nearly twice the risk of ADHD being reported.19 An elevated risk of ADHD subsequent to administration of perinatal oxytocin has also been reported recently.20

    Biederman et al21 suggested that boys may be more susceptible to environmental risk factors than girls. These authors questioned whether the important environmental risk factors may differ between boys and girls, and they identified this as an important subject for additional research. Although most studies have adjusted for some potential confounders, no study to date has considered a comprehensive set of environmental predictors by child’s gender. Furthermore, as outlined by Froehlich and colleagues,22 who reviewed the literature on environmental risk factors and ADHD symptoms, most such studies have limitations, including small sample size, insufficient power, the use of questionnaire reports, parent or caregiver diagnosis of ADHD, recall or social desirability bias, symptoms in toddlers and preschoolers that may not correlate to later diagnostic status, and inconsistencies in diagnosis of ADHD.

    To overcome some of these limitations, the population-based, nested case–control study reported here examines a comprehensive set of possible environmental factors, related to the mother, the pregnancy and delivery of the infant, and the newborn, in association with subsequent diagnosis and treatment with stimulant medication (SM) for ADHD. Extending previous research, it explores whether the risk factors differ between boys and girls.

    Methods

    In August 2003 the Stimulant Regulatory Scheme commenced in Western Australia (WA), where specific guidelines were set for prescribing SM by an authorized prescriber for people with ADHD diagnosed using either the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition or International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision criteria.23 Notification to the Monitoring of Drugs of Dependence System (MODDS) of any person commenced on SM is mandatory.

    Case Population

    For this study, data were extracted from the MODDS on all children and young adults born since January 1, 1980 and <25 years of age who were dispensed SM between August 2003 and December 2007 in WA. A total of 15 733 non-Aboriginal cases were identified. Records of children identified from MODDS were linked to the Midwives Notification System (MNS), a statutory collection of pregnancy, birth, and neonatal information completed by midwives for all births ≥20 weeks’ gestation in WA since 1980.24 We limited our analysis to non-Aboriginal cases born between 1981 and 2003. A total of 12 991 non-Aboriginal cases of ADHD were successfully linked to the MNS. Aboriginal children were not included in this study because ethnic minority groups are less likely to be diagnosed and treated for ADHD.25 Most nonlinked cases were children born outside WA. The study design is illustrated in Fig 1.

    FIGURE 1
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    FIGURE 1

    Study method flowchart.

    Reference (Control) Population

    A random sample of records not linked to MODDS was selected from the MNS. They were frequency matched by year of birth, gender, and socioeconomic status to the case group (2:1); 30 071 controls with pregnancy, birth, and neonatal information were selected. Socioeconomic status was assessed by using the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), which provides a measure of relative socioeconomic status about the area in which a person lives.26

    Data linkage and extraction were performed by staff of the WA Data Linkage Branch. Once linked, the information was deidentified and provided to the researchers.

    Risk and Protective Factors

    The maternal, pregnancy, and newborn factors listed in Tables 2, 3, and 4 were available from the MNS for data analysis (for births from 1980 unless otherwise stated).

    Maternal factors were maternal age (6 categories), marital status (single), birth order (first born), and postcode of residence at delivery. Smoking in pregnancy was available for birth records since 1997.

    Pregnancy, labor, and delivery factors were threatened abortion before 20 weeks’ gestation; maternal UTI; preeclampsia; premature rupture of membranes; onset of labor (spontaneous, induced, no labor); complications of labor, which include precipitous delivery, fetal distress, cord prolapse, and cord tight around neck; delivery presentations (vertex, breech, other); and mode of delivery (spontaneous vaginal, vacuum, forceps, emergency cesarean, and elective cesarean). From 1997, information was available on threatened preterm labor (<37 weeks) and oxytocin for both augmentation and induction of labor.

    Newborn factors were gender, gestational age (GA), birth weight, and Apgar at 5 minutes. We calculated GA by using the algorithm described by Blair et al,27 who included both antenatal and neonatal indicators of gestational duration to obtain a more accurate population measure. We also calculated appropriate for gestational age (AGA), small for gestational age (SGA), and large for gestational age (LGA) to provide a better measure of fetal growth and development.

    Ethics approval for this study was obtained from the WA Department of Health Human Research Ethics Committee and the Human Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia.

    Statistical Analysis

    By using conditional logistic regression to calculate odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs), we initially described the associations between each potential risk factor and the diagnosis of ADHD treated with SM, adjusted only for the matching variables year of birth and SEIFA, examined separately for male and female births (partially adjusted model). For our fully adjusted model we simultaneously included all risk factors from the partially adjusted model of clinical significance and relevance. Two multivariable models were constructed using our limited data set from 1997 to 2002 (Model A) and our complete data set from 1981 to 2002 (Model B), which excluded smoking in pregnancy, preterm labor, and oxytocin use. The data analysis for this paper was generated by using SAS/STAT software, version 9.3 of the SAS System for Windows (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC).28

    Results

    The 2 study groups were well matched by gender, year of birth, and SEIFA (Table 1). Seventy-seven percent were male, and a marginally larger proportion of children with ADHD were in the lower SEIFA group.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 1

    Distribution of Frequency Matching Variables in ADHD Cases and Controls

    Partially Adjusted Model: Factors Related to the Mother, Pregnancy, Labor, and Delivery

    As shown in Table 2, compared with control mothers, mothers of children with ADHD were more likely to be younger, single, and smokers during pregnancy, irrespective of the gender of the child. There was no elevated risk for first-born boys or girls being diagnosed with ADHD. Mothers of children with ADHD were more than twice as likely to have had threatened preterm labor before 37 weeks compared with the control sample (OR = 2.46, 95% CI, 1.73–3.51 for boys; OR = 2.09, 95% CI, 0.90–4.85 for girls).

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 2

    Partially Adjusteda OR and 95% CI of Maternal, Pregnancy, and Labor Risk and Protective Factors Associated With ADHD

    Threatened abortion before 20 weeks’ gestation was also more likely among mothers of cases. There was an elevated risk of ADHD in both boys and girls when mothers had a UTI during pregnancy (OR = 1.37, 95% CI, 1.21–1.55 for boys; OR = 1.51, 95% CI, 1.21–1.90 for girls), or preeclampsia (OR = 1.32, 95% CI, 1.21–1.44 for boys; OR = 1.44, 95% CI, 1.22–1.70 for girls). Induction of labor or no labor and both elective and emergency cesarean deliveries carried a higher risk of subsequently being diagnosed and treated for ADHD. Only oxytocin augmentation and not oxytocin induction of labor appeared to have an effect, protective in female births (OR = 0.58, 95% CI, 0.34–0.98). For fetal distress there was a small (∼10%) elevated risk for both boys and girls. Cord prolapse carried a 2.5 times significant increased risk for ADHD in female births, based on small numbers (OR = 2.50, 95% CI, 1.08–5.77). Breech presentation in boys but not girls was associated with a 17% elevated risk of ADHD (OR = 1.17, 95% CI, 1.04–1.31). There was no elevated risk associated with forceps or vacuum delivery, irrespective of gender.

    Partially Adjusted Model: Factors Related to the Newborn

    Reducing GA carried elevated risk of ADHD for both boys and girls, reaching significance for male births early preterm (<32 weeks), late preterm (33–36 weeks), and early term (37–38 weeks). For early term births, boys had a 12% elevated risk (OR = 1.12, 95% CI, 1.06–1.18), and girls had a 14% elevated risk (OR = 1.14, 95% CI, 1.03–1.27) of ADHD compared with controls (Table 3). There was no increase in risk for postterm deliveries, irrespective of gender. Very low birth weight (<1500 g) carried a small but not significant risk for ADHD, similar for boys and girls. Low birth weights (1500–2499 g) carried an elevated risk for ADHD in boys (OR = 1.16, 95% CI, 1.03–1.30). SGA carried an elevated risk for ADHD for both boys and girls; however, there was no effect for LGA. Low Apgar scores at 5 minutes did not carry an elevated risk for ADHD irrespective of gender.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 3

    Partially Adjusteda OR and 95% CI of Newborn Risk and Protective Factors Associated With ADHD

    Fully Adjusted Model

    The fully adjusted models looked at all relevant study risk factors associated with ADHD (Table 4). The OR associated with single mothers, maternal UTI, preeclampsia, and induced onset of labor remained elevated irrespective of gender. Threatened preterm labor for boys carried a significant risk (OR = 1.83, 95% CI, 1.22–2.75). The OR for smoking in pregnancy remained elevated, with CI excluding unity for both boys (OR = 1.86, 95% CI, 1.53–2.27) and girls (OR = 1.67, 95% CI, 1.07–2.61). Oxytocin used to augment deliveries was protective for girls, although the CI included unity (OR = 0.58; 95% CI, 0.32–1.06). There was a threefold elevated OR for cord prolapse for females only (OR = 2.83; 95% CI, 1.13–7.12). Reducing GA in boys increased the risk of ADHD where CI excluded unity only for early term deliveries. SGA did not remain associated with ADHD in the fully adjusted model.

    View this table:
    • View inline
    • View popup
    TABLE 4

    Fully Adjusted Risk and Protective Factors Associated With ADHD by Gender

    Discussion

    This is the first population study of Australian children with ADHD and one of the largest internationally, looking at potential early risk and protective factors related to the mother, pregnancy, delivery, and the newborn.

    This study clearly identifies an elevated risk of being diagnosed with ADHD and treated with SM among the offspring of young and single mothers, mothers who smoked in pregnancy, and those who had threatened preterm labor, preeclampsia, UTI, and induction of labor, irrespective of the gender of the child. Lower GA and low birth weight for boys were significantly related to ADHD only in our partially adjusted model, although early term deliveries remained significant in our fully adjusted model. Similar to most larger studies in the literature that support the correlation of ADHD by degree of prematurity,16,17,29,30 we also suggest the need to recognize the effect of prematurity even in early term deliveries on subsequent behavior problems including ADHD, and parents requesting early delivery with no obstetric complication should be discouraged.17,31,32 Gustafsson et al14 reported only a weak correlation with perinatal, maternal, and fetal characteristics of children diagnosed with ADHD, and their study lacked power because of a small sample size. Our study was able to expand on their findings using a larger population sample and examining the potential risk factors separately for male and female children. We acknowledge that adjusting for a large number of factors may reduce precision, and real associations may be blurred by the choice of variables included in our model. Although precision was reduced, as evident by wider CIs, we found little change in the estimates of ORs between the partially and fully adjusted models except for lower GA estimates in girls, possibly related to a smaller sample size.

    The gender difference in ADHD is well documented, particularly among children. However, almost all potential risk factors examined were similar for boys and girls, although CI more often excluded unity for boys because of their larger sample size. The exceptions were threatened preterm labor for boys, where there was a twofold elevated risk, and oxytocin augmentation for girls, where there was a reduction in risk of being diagnosed and treated for ADHD.

    Birth order is considered a key environmental factor in child development, shown to be associated with an elevated risk of psychiatric disorders such as ADHD.19 In our fully adjusted model, being first born was not associated with ADHD.18 We found that smoking in pregnancy remained elevated after adjustment for a number of maternal, pregnancy, and birth factors. Chronic exposure to smoking in pregnancy has been extensively studied, with the theoretical model predicting that nicotine receptors modulate dopaminergic activity and dysregulation, which can cause ADHD. Literature supports this consistent association,14,33–37 although several studies question whether the smoking association is all caused by biological effects of tobacco smoke rather than being at least partly explained by confounding factors, which include genetic, socioeconomic, and household factors.38–41 The mechanism of how tobacco smoke affects the fetal brain remains unclear,6,33 but it has been hypothesized that perinatal events resulting in hypoxia may affect brain development and lead to ADHD.11,42,43 Such risk factors include threatened abortion, threatened preterm labor, preeclampsia, prematurity, intrauterine growth retardation, low Apgar scores, and fetal distress.35,44,45 Our study contradicts some of these findings in that we found no association with acute events around delivery except for the rare event of cord prolapse in girls, also suggested in a recent study.11 Maternal preeclampsia and genitourinary tract infections have been associated with other neurodevelopmental disorders, including ADHD,12,46 where an inflammatory cascade affecting fetal brain development has been suggested. In our fully adjusted model, both preeclampsia and maternal genitourinary tract infections were associated with an elevated risk of ADHD irrespective of gender. Induction of labor has been associated with ADHD16 and difficult behavior,20 although no mechanism has been described. We confirmed this finding with an elevated risk irrespective of gender. In our limited data set (Model A) oxytocin augmentation (but not induction) of labor was protective for girls, but CI included unity, and we acknowledge that this may be a chance finding. Oxytocin has recently received more interest in the literature, especially its role in social behavior, although a number of unanswered questions about plausible mechanisms and actions remain.47 Additional studies should investigate this association and provide some neurobiological explanation, including whether a reduced duration of labor is plausible, because it is surprising that such a late intervention as augmentation of labor may potentially have a neuroprotective effect for female births.

    We acknowledge that there are some limitations to our study. First, we had no data on parental mental health; therefore, variables such as family history of ADHD could not be studied.41 Second, our control population consisted of children who were randomly selected from all WA births not prescribed SM from 2003, when the register was first introduced. It is possible that some of the controls may have been on SM before commencement of the register or have ADHD but were not treated with SM at the time. However, because only 1.2% of children <18 years old are commenced on SM in WA, we believe that the number of children with ADHD in the control sample would have little effect on our results, and any effect would have biased the estimates toward the null. Third, data for smoking, threatened preterm labor, oxytocin induction, and oxytocin augmentation of labor were available for only 10% of cases and controls, limiting analyses for these variables. However, we noted little difference when comparing the fully adjusted complete data set (Model B) with the limited data set (Model A). Finally, our study looked at children diagnosed and treated with SM for ADHD, and therefore it is likely to reflect the more severe cases.

    Strengths of this population-based study include the large sample size and the use of record linkage, which removes the possibility of participation and recall bias and loss to follow-up. Furthermore, all the cases met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition and International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision criteria for ADHD and were diagnosed by a pediatrician or psychiatrist, requirements of the prescribing guidelines set out by government legislation in WA.

    Our study is one of the largest population studies on antenatal risk factors published to date, using a robust, reliable data linkage system with only deidentified data, ensuring strict privacy. Importantly, we found that risk associations were generally similar for boys and girls with ADHD. We did not confirm an elevated risk for a number of previously published prenatal, pregnancy, and postnatal factors. However, the risk of ADHD remained elevated in children exposed to maternal smoking, preeclampsia, maternal UTIs, induced labor, and early term deliveries, irrespective of gender. For boys, threatened preterm labor was an elevated risk, and for girls the rare condition of cord prolapse nearly tripled the risk of ADHD. Some events related to the mother, pregnancy, delivery, and newborn contribute to the risk of subsequent ADHD, with few gender differences. Perhaps population studies designed to disentangle possible mechanisms, confounders, or moderators of these risk factors are warranted.

    Acknowledgments

    We acknowledge the excellent support and partnership between the custodians of the Stimulant Notification Register, MNS, Western Australian Data Linkage System, and Health Department of Western Australia. We also acknowledge the WA Developmental Pathways Project and the Australian Research Council for facilitating the linkage (LP100200504). Particular thanks to Professor Fiona Stanley and Dr Rebecca Glauert for their support of this project.

    Footnotes

      • Accepted September 26, 2013.
    • Address correspondence to Desiree Silva, MB, BS, FRACP, MPH, Telethon Institute for Child Health Research, PO Box 855, West Perth, Western Australia 6872. E-mail: desirees{at}ichr.uwa.edu.au
    • Professor Silva conceptualized and designed the study, assisted with the analyses, drafted the manuscript, and revised it critically; Dr Colvin assisted with the study design, carried out the analyses, and reviewed the manuscript; Dr Hagemann assisted with, conceptualized, and designed the study and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Professor Bower conceptualized and designed the study, assisted with the analyses, and assisted with the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

    • FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

    • FUNDING: Supported by National Health and Medical Research Council Program grant 572742 and Fellowship 634341.

    • POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

    References

    1. ↵
      1. Polanczyk G,
      2. de Lima MS,
      3. Horta BL,
      4. Biederman J,
      5. Rohde LA
      . The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942–948pmid:17541055
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    2. ↵
      1. Doshi JA,
      2. Hodgkins P,
      3. Kahle J,
      4. et al.
      Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:990–1002
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    3. ↵
      1. Franke B,
      2. Neale BM,
      3. Faraone SV
      . Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Hum Genet. 2009;126(1):13–50pmid:19384554
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    4. ↵
      1. Lichtenstein P,
      2. Carlström E,
      3. Råstam M,
      4. Gillberg C,
      5. Anckarsäter H
      . The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(11):1357–1363pmid:20686188
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    5. ↵
      1. Nigg J,
      2. Nikolas M,
      3. Burt SA
      . Measured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(9):863–873pmid:20732623
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    6. ↵
      1. Banerjee TD,
      2. Middleton F,
      3. Faraone SV
      . Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96(9):1269–1274pmid:17718779
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Larsson JO,
      2. Larsson H,
      3. Lichtenstein P
      . Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: a longitudinal twin study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43(10):1267–1275pmid:15381894
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    7. ↵
      1. Millichap JG
      . Etiologic classification of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2008;121(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/121/2/e358pmid:18245408
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    8. ↵
      1. Plomp E,
      2. Van Engeland H,
      3. Durston S
      . Understanding genes, environment and their interaction in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: is there a role for neuroimaging? Neuroscience. 2009;164(1):230–240pmid:19619618
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    9. ↵
      1. Thapar A,
      2. Cooper M,
      3. Jefferies R,
      4. Stergiakouli E
      . What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(3):260–265pmid:21903599
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    10. ↵
      1. Getahun D,
      2. Rhoads GG,
      3. Demissie K,
      4. et al
      . In utero exposure to ischemic–hypoxic conditions and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2013;131(1). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/131/1/e53pmid:23230063
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    11. ↵
      1. Mann JR,
      2. McDermott S
      . Are maternal genitourinary infection and pre-eclampsia associated with ADHD in school-aged children? J Atten Disord. 2011;15(8):667–673pmid:20837984
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    12. ↵
      1. St Sauver JL,
      2. Barbaresi WJ,
      3. Katusic SK,
      4. Colligan RC,
      5. Weaver AL,
      6. Jacobsen SJ
      . Early life risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cohort study. Mayo Clin Proc. 2004;79(9):1124–1131pmid:15357033
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    13. ↵
      1. Gustafsson P,
      2. Källén K
      . Perinatal, maternal, and fetal characteristics of children diagnosed with attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder: results from a population-based study utilizing the Swedish Medical Birth Register. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011;53(3):263–268pmid:20964677
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    14. ↵
      1. El Marroun H,
      2. Zeegers M,
      3. Steegers EA,
      4. et al
      . Post-term birth and the risk of behavioural and emotional problems in early childhood. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(3):773–781pmid:22552873
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    15. ↵
      1. Halmøy A,
      2. Klungsøyr K,
      3. Skjærven R,
      4. Haavik J
      . Pre- and perinatal risk factors in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(5):474–481pmid:22200325
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    16. ↵
      1. Lindström K,
      2. Lindblad F,
      3. Hjern A
      . Preterm birth and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildren. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5):858–865pmid:21502231
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    17. ↵
      1. Berger I,
      2. Felsenthal-Berger N
      . Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and birth order. J Child Neurol. 2009;24(6):692–696pmid:19211923
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    18. ↵
      Do firstborn children have an increased risk of ADHD? [Database on the Internet]. 2012. Available at: http://jad.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/07/23/1087054712445066
    19. ↵
      1. Kurth L,
      2. Haussmann R
      . Perinatal Pitocin as an early ADHD biomarker: neurodevelopmental risk? J Atten Disord. 2011;15(5):423–431pmid:21527574
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    20. ↵
      1. Biederman J,
      2. Faraone SV,
      3. Monuteaux MC
      . Differential effect of environmental adversity by gender: Rutter’s index of adversity in a group of boys and girls with and without ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(9):1556–1562pmid:12202277
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    21. ↵
      1. Froehlich TE,
      2. Anixt JS,
      3. Loe IM,
      4. Chirdkiatgumchai V,
      5. Kuan L,
      6. Gilman RC
      . Update on environmental risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2011;13(5):333–344pmid:21779823
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    22. ↵
      1. Department of Health
      . Western Australia Stimulant Regulatory Scheme 2011 Annual Report. Perth, Australia: Pharmaceutical Services Branch, Public Health and Clinical Division, Department of Health; 2012
    23. ↵
      1. Nguyen N,
      2. Gee V,
      3. Le M
      . Perinatal Statistics in Western Australia, 2007: Twenty-Fifth Annual Report of the Western Australian Midwives’ Notification System. Perth, Australia: Western Australia Department of Health; 2008
    24. ↵
      1. Morgan PL,
      2. Staff J,
      3. Hillemeier MM,
      4. Farkas G,
      5. Maczuga S
      . Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis from kindergarten to eighth grade. Pediatrics. 2013;132(1):85–93pmid:23796743
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    25. ↵
      1. Technical Paper:
      Census of Population and Housing: Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA). Cat 2039.0.55.001. Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2001
    26. ↵
      1. Blair E,
      2. Liu Y,
      3. Cosgrove P
      . Choosing the best estimate of gestational age from routinely collected population-based perinatal data. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2004;18(4):270–276pmid:15255880
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    27. ↵
      1. SAS/STAT [computer program]
      . Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc; 2010
    28. ↵
      1. Bhutta AT,
      2. Cleves MA,
      3. Casey PH,
      4. Cradock MM,
      5. Anand KJ
      . Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2002;288(6):728–737pmid:12169077
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    29. ↵
      1. Linnet KM,
      2. Wisborg K,
      3. Agerbo E,
      4. Secher NJ,
      5. Thomsen PH,
      6. Henriksen TB
      . Gestational age, birth weight, and the risk of hyperkinetic disorder. Arch Dis Child. 2006;91(8):655–660pmid:16754656
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    30. ↵
      1. Clark SL,
      2. Miller DD,
      3. Belfort MA,
      4. Dildy GA,
      5. Frye DK,
      6. Meyers JA
      . Neonatal and maternal outcomes associated with elective term delivery. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2009;200:156, e1–e4
      OpenUrlPubMed
    31. ↵
      1. Robinson M,
      2. Whitehouse AJ,
      3. Zubrick SR,
      4. et al
      . Delivery at 37 weeks’ gestation is associated with a higher risk for child behavioural problems. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol. 2013;53(2):143–151pmid:23173808
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    32. ↵
      1. Biederman J,
      2. Faraone SV
      . Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2005;366(9481):237–248pmid:16023516
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Froehlich TE,
      2. Lanphear BP,
      3. Auinger P,
      4. et al
      . Association of tobacco and lead exposures with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2009;124(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/124/6/e1054pmid:19933729
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    33. ↵
      1. Milberger S,
      2. Biederman J,
      3. Faraone SV,
      4. Guite J,
      5. Tsuang MT
      . Pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: issues of gene–environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(1):65–75pmid:8988797
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
      1. Motlagh MG,
      2. Sukhodolsky DG,
      3. Landeros-Weisenberger A,
      4. et al
      . Adverse effects of heavy prenatal maternal smoking on attentional control in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2011;15(7):593–603pmid:20616372
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    34. ↵
      1. Neuman RJ,
      2. Lobos E,
      3. Reich W,
      4. Henderson CA,
      5. Sun LW,
      6. Todd RD
      . Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(12):1320–1328pmid:17157268
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    35. ↵
      1. Langley K,
      2. Heron J,
      3. Smith GD,
      4. Thapar A
      . Maternal and paternal smoking during pregnancy and risk of ADHD symptoms in offspring: testing for intrauterine effects. Am J Epidemiol. 2012;176(3):261–268pmid:22791738
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Lindblad F,
      2. Hjern A
      . ADHD after fetal exposure to maternal smoking. Nicotine Tob Res. 2010;12(4):408–415pmid:20176681
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Obel C,
      2. Olsen J,
      3. Henriksen TB,
      4. et al
      . Is maternal smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for hyperkinetic disorder? Findings from a sibling design. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(2):338–345pmid:21075808
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    36. ↵
      1. Thapar A,
      2. Rutter M
      . Do prenatal risk factors cause psychiatric disorder? Be wary of causal claims. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(2):100–101pmid:19648537
      OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    37. ↵
      1. Lou HC
      . Etiology and pathogenesis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): significance of prematurity and perinatal hypoxic–haemodynamic encephalopathy. Acta Paediatr. 1996;85(11):1266–1271pmid:8955450
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    38. ↵
      1. Boksa P,
      2. El-Khodor BF
      . Birth insult interacts with stress at adulthood to alter dopaminergic function in animal models: possible implications for schizophrenia and other disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(1–2):91–101pmid:12732226
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    39. ↵
      1. Chandola CA,
      2. Robling MR,
      3. Peters TJ,
      4. Melville-Thomas G,
      5. McGuffin P
      . Pre- and perinatal factors and the risk of subsequent referral for hyperactivity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(6):1077–1090pmid:1400688
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    40. ↵
      1. Li J,
      2. Olsen J,
      3. Vestergaard M,
      4. Obel C
      . Low Apgar scores and risk of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Pediatr. 2011;158(5):775–779pmid:21238979
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    41. ↵
      1. Mann JR,
      2. McDermott S,
      3. Bao H,
      4. Bersabe A
      . Maternal genitourinary infection and risk of cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(4):282–288pmid:19191825
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    42. ↵
      1. Young LJ,
      2. Flanagan-Cato LM
      . Editorial comment: oxytocin, vasopressin and social behavior. Horm Behav. 2012;61(3):227–229pmid:22443808
      OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    • Copyright © 2014 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    PreviousNext
    Back to top

    Advertising Disclaimer »

    In this issue

    Pediatrics
    Vol. 133, Issue 1
    1 Jan 2014
    • Table of Contents
    • Index by author
    View this article with LENS
    PreviousNext
    Email Article

    Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

    NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

    Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
    Environmental Risk Factors by Gender Associated With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
    (Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
    (Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
    CAPTCHA
    This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
    Request Permissions
    Article Alerts
    Log in
    You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
    Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
    Citation Tools
    Environmental Risk Factors by Gender Associated With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
    Desiree Silva, Lyn Colvin, Erika Hagemann, Carol Bower
    Pediatrics Jan 2014, 133 (1) e14-e22; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-1434

    Citation Manager Formats

    • BibTeX
    • Bookends
    • EasyBib
    • EndNote (tagged)
    • EndNote 8 (xml)
    • Medlars
    • Mendeley
    • Papers
    • RefWorks Tagged
    • Ref Manager
    • RIS
    • Zotero
    Share
    Environmental Risk Factors by Gender Associated With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
    Desiree Silva, Lyn Colvin, Erika Hagemann, Carol Bower
    Pediatrics Jan 2014, 133 (1) e14-e22; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2013-1434
    del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
    Print
    Download PDF
    Insight Alerts
    • Table of Contents

    Jump to section

    • Article
      • Abstract
      • Methods
      • Results
      • Discussion
      • Acknowledgments
      • Footnotes
      • References
    • Figures & Data
    • Info & Metrics
    • Comments

    Related Articles

    • No related articles found.
    • PubMed
    • Google Scholar

    Cited By...

    • Maternal Smoking and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Offspring: A Meta-analysis
    • Hypertensive disorders of pregnancy and risk of neurodevelopmental disorders in the offspring: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol
    • Birth Spacing and Risk of Autism and Other Neurodevelopmental Disabilities: A Systematic Review
    • Preterm Birth and Poor Fetal Growth as Risk Factors of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
    • Medical Augmentation of Labor and the Risk of ADHD in Offspring: A Population-Based Study
    • Parental Smoking During Pregnancy and ADHD in Children: The Danish National Birth Cohort
    • Google Scholar

    More in this TOC Section

    • Clinical Impact of a Diagnostic Gastrointestinal Panel in Children
    • Intrapartum Group B Streptococcal Prophylaxis and Childhood Allergic Disorders
    • Changes in Neurodevelopmental Outcomes From Age 2 to 10 Years for Children Born Extremely Preterm
    Show more Article

    Similar Articles

    Subjects

    • Fetus/Newborn Infant
      • Fetus/Newborn Infant
    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
      • Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

    Keywords

    • ADHD
    • risk factors
    • gestational age
    • birth weight
    • smoking in pregnancy
    • Journal Info
    • Editorial Board
    • Editorial Policies
    • Overview
    • Licensing Information
    • Authors/Reviewers
    • Author Guidelines
    • Submit My Manuscript
    • Open Access
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Librarians
    • Institutional Subscriptions
    • Usage Stats
    • Support
    • Contact Us
    • Subscribe
    • Resources
    • Media Kit
    • About
    • International Access
    • Terms of Use
    • Privacy Statement
    • FAQ
    • AAP.org
    • shopAAP
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
    • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
    • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
    • RSS
    American Academy of Pediatrics

    © 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics