Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Infant Growth and Child Cognition at 3 Years of Age

Mandy B. Belfort, Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman, Janet W. Rich-Edwards, Ken P. Kleinman, Emily Oken and Matthew W. Gillman
Pediatrics September 2008, 122 (3) e689-e695; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-0500
Mandy B. Belfort
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Janet W. Rich-Edwards
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ken P. Kleinman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Emily Oken
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Matthew W. Gillman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

BACKGROUND. Infancy is a critical period for brain development. Few studies have examined the extent to which infant weight gain is associated with later neurodevelopmental outcomes in healthy populations.

OBJECTIVE. The purpose of this work was to examine associations of infant weight gain from birth to 6 months with child cognitive and visual-motor skills at 3 years of age.

PATIENTS AND METHODS. We studied 872 participants in Project Viva, an ongoing prospective, longitudinal, prebirth cohort. We abstracted birth weight from the medical chart and weighed infants at 6 months of age. We used the 2000 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts to derive weight-for-age z scores. Our primary predictor was infant weight gain, defined as the weight-for-age z score at 6 months adjusted for the weight-for-age z score at birth. At 3 years of age, we measured child cognition with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III and visual-motor skills with the Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities.

RESULTS. Mean Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III score was 104.2, and mean Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities test score was 102.8. Mean birth weight z score was 0.21, and mean 6-month weight z score was 0.39. In multiple linear regression adjusted for child age, gender, gestational age, breastfeeding duration, primary language, and race/ethnicity; maternal age, parity, smoking status, and cognition; and parental education and income level, we found no association of infant weight gain with child Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III score (−0.4 points per z score weight gain increment, 95% confidence interval −1.3, 0.6) or total Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities standard score (−0.4 points, 95% confidence interval −1.2, 0.5).

CONCLUSIONS. Slower infant weight gain was not associated with poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes in healthy, term-born 3-year-old children. These results should aid in determining optimal growth patterns in infants to balance risks and benefits of health outcomes through the life course.

  • infant growth
  • neurodevelopment

The third trimester of pregnancy through ≥2 years postterm is a critical period for brain development.1 During that time, the brain develops rapidly through the processes of neurogenesis, axonal and dendritic growth, synaptogenesis, cell death, synaptic pruning, myelination, and gliogenesis.2 These developmental processes build on each other over time so that a small disruption in any of the processes may have wide and long-lasting effects on brain structure and function.3

In ecological4 and interventional5,6 studies in developing countries with extensive childhood malnutrition, studies of children with failure to thrive (FTT),7 and studies of children born preterm,8–10 slower infant weight gain is associated with poorer cognition later in life. However, there are few studies of infant weight gain and cognitive development in healthy populations. One study11 from the United Kingdom found that lower infant weight between 9 and 24 months was associated with only marginally lower cognitive and achievement test scores at 10 years of age. Another United Kingdom study12 found a small, linear relationship of slower weight gain from birth to 8 weeks of age with lower IQ at 8 years of age but no appreciable relationship of weight gain from 8 weeks to 9 months of age with later IQ.

It is important to know whether slower infant weight gain is associated with poorer cognition in healthy populations of infants, because rapid weight gain may carry some harms, including an increase in the risk of obesity and related disorders later in life.13–19 Promoting slower infant weight gain might help to prevent these disorders, which are rising rapidly, even among the very young.20

The aim of this study was to examine the extent to which infant weight gain in the first 6 months of life is associated with cognition and visual-motor skills at 3 years of age. We hypothesized that slower infant weight gain would not be associated with poorer cognitive or visual-motor skills in generally healthy, term-born children.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Study Design and Participants

We studied participants in Project Viva, a prospective, longitudinal cohort study originally designed to examine multiple prenatal factors in relation to outcomes of pregnancy and child health. We recruited pregnant women into Project Viva at their first prenatal visit at Harvard Vanguard Medical Associates, a large multispecialty group practice in eastern Massachusetts. Exclusion criteria included multiple gestation, inability to answer questions in English, plans to move from the area before delivery, and gestational age >22 completed weeks at the initial prenatal clinical appointment. Additional details of recruitment and follow-up have been published elsewhere.21 Human subject committees of Harvard Pilgrim Health Care, Brigham and Women's Hospital, and Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (Boston, MA) approved the study protocols, and mothers of all of the participating children gave informed consent.

For this analysis, we included children with complete exposure and outcome data at birth, 6 months, and 3 years, and excluded children born <37 completed weeks’ gestation. Of the 2128 women who delivered a live infant, 1579 had children eligible for the 3-year follow-up, and we performed in-person examinations on 1292 (82%). Of these 1292 children, we excluded 83 who were born before 37 completed weeks’ gestation and 311 with missing exposure and/or outcome data. Thus, our sample for this analysis was 872 children and their mothers.

Measurements

Infant Anthropometry

Research assistants abstracted birth weight from the medical chart, weighed infants at 6 months of age with a digital scale (Seca Model 881, Seca Corporation, Hamburg, Germany), and measured infants’ length at 6 months using a Shorr measuring board (Shorr Productions, Olney, MD). We also performed medical chart review to obtain data on participant weight at 8 weeks of age.

Child Cognition

Research assistants followed a standardized protocol to administer the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III (PPVT-III) and the Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities (WRAVMA) to children at the 3-year study visit. The PPVT-III is a test of receptive language and is highly correlated with intelligence tests such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III (Pearson r = 0.90 with the full-scale IQ score).22 The WRAVMA measures visual-motor, visual-spatial, and fine motor skills and is composed of drawing, matching, and pegboard tests.23

Covariates

We collected data from mothers regarding parental and child demographic, social, economic, and health information through self-administered questionnaires and interviews at study visits during pregnancy, shortly after delivery, and when the child was 6 months and 3 years old. Details about the source of information for most of these variables have been described elsewhere.21,24 We measured maternal cognition using the PPVT-III at the child's 3-year study visit.

Analysis

Our primary outcomes were the PPVT-III and total WRAVMA standard scores, and our secondary outcomes were the WRAVMA drawing, matching, and pegboard scores. We used the 2000 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts25,26 to derive weight and weight-for-length z scores at birth and 6 months of age. Our primary predictor was the weight z score at 6 months of age, adjusted for the birth weight z score. We refer to this expression as “infant weight gain from birth to 6 months of age,” because it is algebraically identical to the change in weight z score from birth to 6 months, adjusted for the birth weight z score. We chose our approach for ease of statistical modeling and interpretation. In addition, to allow comparison with another study,12 we examined infant weight gain from birth to 8 weeks and from 8 weeks to 6 months of age as predictors of our cognitive outcomes.

We used multivariable linear regression to estimate associations of infant weight gain with our cognitive outcomes. To control for confounding, in our final model we included parental and child covariates that are associated with infant weight gain and/or child cognition, including the child's gestational age, breastfeeding duration, race or ethnicity, and English as a second language status; the mother's age, parity, smoking status, and PPVT-III score; the parents’ educational levels; and household income. We also controlled for child gender and age at cognitive testing. Additional adjustment for maternal gestational weight gain, depression, or parental marital status did not change the estimates, so we did not include them in our final model. We performed all of the data analyses with SAS 9.1 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC).

RESULTS

Table 1 lists participant characteristics. The mean (SD) PPVT-III score at 3 years of age was 104.2 points (14.4 points), similar to the standardized test mean of 100 points and SD of 15 points. The range of PPVT-III scores was 64 to 148 points. The total WRAVMA standard score is the sum of the WRAVMA drawing, matching, and pegboard test scores, standardized to a mean of 100 and SD of 15 points. In our sample, the mean (SD) total WRAVMA standard score was 102.8 (11.2), and the range was 57 to 151. The mean (SD) weight z score at birth was 0.21 (0.94) and at 6 months was 0.39 (0.96). Mean (SD) breastfeeding duration was 6.7 months (4.5 months).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1

Characteristics of 872 Participants in Project Viva

Compared with the 1579 mother-child pairs who were eligible for the 3-year follow-up, mothers in this analysis were more likely to be of white race (76% vs 69%), better educated (73% vs 68% with college or graduate degree), and more likely to have an annual household income exceeding $70 000 (67% vs 62%) but were of similar age (32.6 vs 32.1 years). Children in this analysis had similar mean birth weight (3.58 vs 3.55 kg) and gestational age (39.9 vs 39.8 weeks) compared with all of the eligible children.

In unadjusted linear regression analyses (Table 2), we found that, for each z score increment in birth weight, the PPVT-III score was 2.3 points higher (95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.3 to 3.3 points), and the total WRAVMA standard score was minimally higher (0.6 points [95% CI: −0.2 to 1.5 points]). Similarly, for each z score increment in birth weight for gestational age (a measure of fetal growth), the PPVT-III score was 2.4 points higher (95% CI: 1.4 to 3.5 points), and the total WRAVMA standard score was 0.7 points higher (95% CI: −0.1 to 1.6 points). There was no association of infant weight z score at 6 months with PPVT-III score (−0.1 points per z score increment [95% CI: −1.1 to 0.9 points]) or total WRAVMA standard score (−0.4 points per z score increment [95% CI: −1.2 to 0.5 points]). There was also no association of infant weight-for-length z score at birth or 6 months with the outcomes.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 2

Unadjusted Associations (95% CI and P Value) of Infant Size and Growth Variables With Cognitive Outcomes at 3 Years of Age

Table 3 shows the results of our multivariable linear regression models. Weight z score at 6 months adjusted for birth weight z score represents infant weight gain from birth to 6 months. For each z score increment in infant weight gain, the PPVT-III score was 1.1 points lower (95% CI: −2.2 to 0.0 points), and the total WRAVMA standard score was 0.9 points lower (95% CI: −1.7 to 0.0 points). Adjustment for gestational age (model 2) minimally affected these estimates, whereas additional adjustment for breastfeeding duration (model 3) attenuated the association of infant weight gain with PPVT-III score to −0.4 points (95% CI: −1.5 to 0.7 points). Adjustment for parental and child covariates (model 4) minimally affected the association of infant weight gain with the cognitive test scores. In our fully adjusted model (model 4), the association of breastfeeding duration with the cognitive outcomes was 0.3 PPVT-III points per month breastfed (95% CI: 0.1 to 0.5 points) and 0.0 total standard WRAVMA points per month breastfed (95% CI: −0.2 to 0.2 points).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 3

Adjusted Associations of Infant Weight Gain From Birth to 6 Months With Child Cognitive Scores at 3 Years of Age

We performed secondary analyses examining infant growth in weight for length, a measure of adiposity. After adjustment for the same covariates as in model 4, associations of infant growth in weight for length with PPVT-III and WRAVMA scores were similar to associations of infant weight gain with PPVT-III and WRAVMA scores (data not shown).

In another secondary analysis, we divided our cohort into gender-specific deciles of infant weight gain and estimated the mean PPVT-III score in each decile (Fig⇓). Adjusting for the same covariates as in model 4, we found that mean PPVT-III scores were 104.0 and 101.3 points in the lowest and highest deciles of weight gain, respectively.

FIGURE
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
FIGURE

Estimated Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III ( PPVT-III) score (standard error) within deciles of infant weight z-score at 6 months (A) and 8 weeks (B). Range of 6 month weight z-scores within deciles for boys: D1(−2.1, −0.9), D3(−0.5, − 0.2), D5(0.0, 0.3), D7(0.5, 0.8), D9(1.1, 1.4), D10 (1.4, 3.2); and for girls D1(−2.6, −0.8), D3(−0.4, −0.1), D5(−0.2, 0.4), D7(0.7, 1.0), D9(1.4, 1.8), D10(1.8, 3.6). Range of 8 week weight z-scores within deciles for boys: D1(−2.2, −0.5), D3(−0.1, 0.1), D5(0.4, 0.6), D7(0.9, 1.1), D9(1.5, 1.8), D10(1.8, 4.3); and for girls D1(−2.1, −0.5), D3(−0.1, 0.1), D5(0.4, 0.6), D7(0.8, 1.1), D9(1.4, 1.8), D10(1.8, 3.1). Estimates are adjusted for child birth weight z score, sex, age at cognitive assessment, gestational age, breastfeeding duration, race/ethnicity, and English as a second language status; maternal age, parity, smoking status, and PPVT-III score; parental educational levels; and annual household income.

To allow comparison with another study,12 we examined infant weight gain in 2 separate intervals: from birth to 8 weeks and from 8 weeks to 6 months of age. Unlike the other study, we did not find that faster weight gain from birth to 8 weeks predicted higher 3-year PPVT-III scores (Table 4). In fact, the adjusted PPVT-III score in the lowest decile was slightly higher, at 104.5 points, than in the highest decile, at 102.7 points. Likewise, we found no association of weight gain from 8 weeks to 6 months with PPVT-III score (Table 4).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 4

Adjusted Associations of Infant Weight Gain in 2 Intervals With Cognitive Test Scores at 3 Years of Age

DISCUSSION

We found that, in our cohort of generally healthy, term-born children, slower infant weight gain in the first 6 months of life was not associated with lower cognitive or visual-motor test scores at 3 years of age. We also found no evidence that children with the slowest weight gain had poorer test scores than children with the fastest weight gain.

Most investigations of infant weight gain and later cognition in developed countries have focused on children with and without FTT7 and have generally found that the children with FTT have lower IQ and other cognitive and achievement test scores. However, most of those studies draw cases of FTT from inpatient wards, specialty clinics, or primary care clinics serving predominantly poor children, so the results are not generalizable to the general population of infants. Some population-based studies27–29 of FTT and later cognition focus exclusively on economically deprived communities and are also limited because they do not account adequately for factors such as the infant's gestational age, breastfeeding status, or for maternal cognition. These factors may be important predictors of postnatal weight gain and childhood neurodevelopment even within the range of “normal” birth weights30–33 and, thus, potential confounders of the relationship between infant weight gain and later cognition.

The results of our study are similar to 2 others11,12 from the United Kingdom that, like ours, examined child neurodevelopment across a spectrum of infant weight gain in a generally healthy population, rather than exclusively in children with and without FTT. In 1 of those studies,11 cognitive and school achievement test scores (standardized test mean: 50 points [SD 10 points]) at 10 years of age were 0.3 to 0.7 points lower for each SD score of slower infant weight gain; and in the other,12 adjusted IQ at 8 years of age was 0.2 points lower per SD score of slower weight gain from birth to 9 months. These effect sizes and our own are too small to support an effect of infant weight gain on neurodevelopment in a healthy population that is of clinical or public health significance.

One of the United Kingdom studies12 also examined weight gain separately from birth to 8 weeks and from 8 weeks to 9 months of age. In contrast to our findings, those authors found a small, linear association of weight gain from birth to 8 weeks of age with IQ at 8 years of age (0.8 points per SD weight gain [95% CI: 0.4 to 1.3 points]). They also found that the IQ of infants in the slowest weight gain category (less than −1.5 SDs) was ∼3 points lower than the IQ of those in the highest weight gain category (>1.5 SDs). In secondary analyses of our data, we found no association of early weight gain with our cognitive outcome, the PPVT-III score, which is highly correlated with IQ. In addition, in our study, children in the lowest category of weight gain from birth to 8 weeks did not have lower PPVT-III scores than children in the highest category of weight gain. Thus, one must consider the possibility that their findings were attributable to chance.

Also in contrast to our findings, studies of preterm infants8–10 suggest that slower infant weight gain does lead to poorer cognitive development in that population. One possible explanation for the adverse effect of slower infant weight gain on neurodevelopment in preterm infants is that they experience a degree of undernutrition and energy imbalance in the first months after birth34 that is substantially greater than healthy, term infants in developed countries typically experience. In addition, the preterm brain may be more vulnerable to the effects of poor weight gain.35,36 Alternatively, there may be shared determinants of poor weight gain and neurodevelopmental impairment in preterm infants, such as perinatal neurologic injury, that lead both to feeding difficulties and to impaired cognitive function.

Mounting evidence suggests that rapid infant weight gain may have harmful consequences, including increasing the risk of obesity,13–15 high blood pressure,16,19 and insulin resistance.17 These disorders are on the rise and seem to have origins early in life.18,37 Promoting slower infant weight gain may be an effective strategy to prevent these disorders, and our results suggest that, within the range of weight gain that we examined, there do not seem to be harmful effects of slower infant weight gain on cognition at 3 years of age in generally healthy, term-born children. However, other potential adverse effects of slower infant weight gain, such as increased susceptibility to infection38 and decreased adult height,14 also need to be evaluated in healthy populations of infants.

An important strength of our study is that we carefully measured and controlled for numerous potentially confounding covariates, including gestational age, breastfeeding duration, maternal intelligence, and socioeconomic variables. However, we did not have specific information about infant-caregiver interaction patterns or family functioning, so residual confounding is possible. Our findings should be applied to generally healthy term-born children, not preterm children or children with extremely poor weight gain, such as those with FTT. We confirmed others’ findings in healthy populations that higher birth weight is associated with higher cognitive test scores,30–33 suggesting that ours is a representative cohort, although generalizability of our findings may be limited by the relatively high socioeconomic status of our participants and the preferential loss to follow-up of participants in lower socioeconomic status and minority racial and ethnic groups. In addition, although we assessed cognition and visual-motor skills using well-validated instruments, our study cannot exclude associations of infant weight gain with other aspects of neurodevelopment, such as executive function and behavior. Finally, although cognitive function at 3 years of age is correlated with later intelligence, testing at ≥5 years of age is likely to yield a more stable measure of intelligence.39 We are currently collecting cognitive data on our cohort at 7 years of age.

CONCLUSIONS

In this study of healthy term-born children, we found that slower infant weight gain is not associated with poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes at 3 years of age. These results should aid in determining optimal growth patterns in infants to balance risks and benefits of health outcomes through the life course.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants HD 34568, HL 64925, and HL 68041, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention task order 0957-007, Harvard Medical School, and Harvard Pilgrim Health Care Foundation. We also thank the staff and participants of Project Viva.

Footnotes

    • Accepted April 16, 2008.
  • Address correspondence to Mandy B. Belfort, MD, MPH, Children's Hospital Boston, Division of Newborn Medicine, Hunnewell 437, 300 Longwood Ave, Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: mandy.belfort{at}childrens.harvard.edu
  • The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

  • What's Known on This Subject

    In children with FTT and children born preterm, slower weight gain is associated with poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes. Little is know about the extent to which infant weight gain is associated with neurodevelopment in healthy, term-born populations.

    What This Study Adds

    Slower weight gain was not associated with poorer neurodevelopment in healthy, term-born 3-year-old children. Children with the slowest weight gain did not have lower cognitive test scores than children with the fastest weight gain.

FTT—failure to thrive • PPVT-III—Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III • WRAVMA—Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities • CI—confidence interval

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    Smart JL. “Malnutrition, learning and behavior”: 25 years on from the MIT symposium. Proc Nutr Soc.1993;52 (1):189– 199
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    Webb SJ, Monk CS, Nelson CA. Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development. Dev Neuropsychol.2001;19 (2):147– 171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet.2007;369 (9555):60– 70
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet.2007;369 (9556):145– 157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet.2005;366 (9499):1804– 1807
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Husaini MA, Karyadi L, Husaini YK, Sandjaja, Karyadi D, Pollitt E. Developmental effects of short-term supplementary feeding in nutritionally-at-risk Indonesian infants. Am J Clin Nutr.1991;54 (5):799– 804
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Corbett SS, Drewett RF. To what extent is failure to thrive in infancy associated with poorer cognitive development? A review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry.2004;45 (3):641– 654
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Casey PH, Whiteside-Mansell L, Barrett K, Bradley RH, Gargus R. Impact of prenatal and/or postnatal growth problems in low birth weight preterm infants on school-age outcomes: an 8-year longitudinal evaluation. Pediatrics.2006;118 (3):1078– 1086
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. Hack M, Merkatz IR, Gordon D, Jones PK, Fanaroff AA. The prognostic significance of postnatal growth in very low-birth weight infants. Am J Obstet Gynecol.1982;143 (6):693– 699
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    Ehrenkranz RA, Dusick AM, Vohr BR, Wright LL, Wrage LA, Poole WK. Growth in the neonatal intensive care unit influences neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics.2006;117 (4):1253– 1261
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Corbett SS, Drewett RF, Durham M, Tymms P, Wright CM. The relationship between birthweight, weight gain in infancy, and educational attainment in childhood. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol.2007;21 (1):57– 64
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Emond AM, Blair PS, Emmett PM, Drewett RF. Weight faltering in infancy and IQ levels at 8 years in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Pediatrics.2007;120 (4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/120/4/e1051
  13. ↵
    Baird J, Fisher D, Lucas P, Kleijnen J, Roberts H, Law C. Being big or growing fast: systematic review of size and growth in infancy and later obesity. BMJ.2005;331 (7522):929
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Ong KK, Loos RJ. Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta Paediatr.2006;95 (8):904– 908
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Monteiro PO, Victora CG. Rapid growth in infancy and childhood and obesity in later life: a systematic review. Obes Rev.2005;6 (2):143– 154
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Huxley RR, Shiell AW, Law CM. The role of size at birth and postnatal catch-up growth in determining systolic blood pressure: a systematic review of the literature. J Hypertens.2000;18 (7):815– 831
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Soto N, Bazaes RA, Pena V, et al. Insulin sensitivity and secretion are related to catch-up growth in small-for-gestational-age infants at age 1 year: results from a prospective cohort. J Clin Endocrinol Metab.2003;88 (8):3645– 3650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Singhal A, Lucas A. Early origins of cardiovascular disease: is there a unifying hypothesis? Lancet.2004;363 (9421):1642– 1645
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Rich-Edwards J, Kleinman KP, Gillman MW. Size at birth, infant growth, and blood pressure at three years of age. J Pediatr.2007;151 (6):670– 674
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Kim J, Peterson KE, Scanlon KS, et al. Trends in overweight from 1980 through 2001 among preschool-aged children enrolled in a health maintenance organization. Obesity (Silver Spring).2006;14 (7):1107– 1112
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Gillman MW, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Lieberman ES, Kleinman KP, Lipshultz SE. Maternal age and other predictors of newborn blood pressure. J Pediatr.2004;144 (2):240– 245
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    Williams KT, Wang JJ. Technical References to the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed. (PPVT-III). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc;1997
  23. ↵
    Adams W, Sheslow D. WRAVMA (Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc;1995
  24. ↵
    Oken E, Huh SY, Taveras EM, Rich-Edwards JW, Gillman MW. Associations of maternal prenatal smoking with child adiposity and blood pressure. Obes Res.2005;13 (11):2021– 2028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, et al. CDC growth charts: United States. Adv Data.2000;(314):1– 27
  26. ↵
    Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, et al. CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat 11.2002;(246):1– 190
  27. ↵
    Boddy J, Skuse D, Andrews B. The developmental sequelae of nonorganic failure to thrive. J Child Psychol Psychiatry.2000;41 (8):1003– 1014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. Skuse D, Pickles A, Wolke D, Reilly S. Postnatal growth and mental development: evidence for a “sensitive period.” J Child Psychol Psychiatry.1994;35 (3):521– 545
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    Corbett SS, Drewett RF, Wright CM. Does a fall down a centile chart matter? The growth and developmental sequelae of mild failure to thrive. Acta Paediatr.1996;85 (11):1278– 1283
    OpenUrlPubMed
  30. ↵
    Matte TD, Bresnahan M, Begg MD, Susser E. Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. BMJ.2001;323 (7308):310– 314
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth ME. Birth weight and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort: longitudinal population based study. BMJ.2001;322 (7280):199– 203
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Pattie A, Rush MA, Whalley LJ, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive function at age 11 years: the Scottish Mental Survey 1932. Arch Dis Child.2001;85 (3):189– 196
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Sørensen HT, Sabroe S, Olsen J, Rothman KJ, Gillman MW, Fischer P. Birth weight and cognitive function in young adult life: historical cohort study. BMJ.1997;315 (7105):401– 403
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    Ehrenkranz RA, Younes N, Lemons JA, et al. Longitudinal growth of hospitalized very low birth weight infants. Pediatrics.1999;104 (2 pt 1):280– 289
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Lucas A. Long-term programming effects of early nutrition: implications for the preterm infant. J Perinatol.2005;25 (suppl 2):S2– S6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Yeung MY. Postnatal growth, neurodevelopment and altered adiposity after preterm birth–from a clinical nutrition perspective. Acta Paediatr.2006;95 (8):909– 917
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Oken E, Gillman MW. Fetal origins of obesity. Obes Res.2003;11 (4):496– 506
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Victora CG, Barros FC, Horta BL, Martorell R. Short-term benefits of catch-up growth for small-for-gestational-age infants. Int J Epidemiol.2001;30 (6):1325– 1330
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Sattler JM. Assessment of Children: Cognitive Applications. 4th ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler, Inc;2001
  40. ↵
    Oken E, Kleinman KP, Rich-Edwards J, Gillman MW. A nearly continuous measure of birth weight for gestational age using a United States national reference. BMC Pediatr.2003;3 (6)
  • Copyright © 2008 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
PreviousNext
Back to top

Advertising Disclaimer »

In this issue

Pediatrics
Vol. 122, Issue 3
September 2008
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
View this article with LENS
PreviousNext
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Infant Growth and Child Cognition at 3 Years of Age
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Request Permissions
Article Alerts
Log in
You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Infant Growth and Child Cognition at 3 Years of Age
Mandy B. Belfort, Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman, Janet W. Rich-Edwards, Ken P. Kleinman, Emily Oken, Matthew W. Gillman
Pediatrics Sep 2008, 122 (3) e689-e695; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2008-0500

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Infant Growth and Child Cognition at 3 Years of Age
Mandy B. Belfort, Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman, Janet W. Rich-Edwards, Ken P. Kleinman, Emily Oken, Matthew W. Gillman
Pediatrics Sep 2008, 122 (3) e689-e695; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2008-0500
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Print
Download PDF
Insight Alerts
  • Table of Contents

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • PATIENTS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • CONCLUSIONS
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Maternal Deworming Research Study (MADRES) protocol: a double-blind, placebo-controlled randomised trial to determine the effectiveness of deworming in the immediate postpartum period
  • Impact of Neonatal Growth on IQ and Behavior at Early School Age
  • Early rapid growth: no association with later cognitive functions in children born not small for gestational age
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Health Outcomes in Young Children Following Pertussis Vaccination During Pregnancy
  • Rural-Urban Differences in Changes and Effects of Tobacco 21 in Youth E-cigarette Use
  • Neonatal SARS-CoV-2 Infections in Breastfeeding Mothers
Show more Articles

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
    • Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
    • Cognition/Language/Learning Disorders
  • Journal Info
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Policies
  • Overview
  • Licensing Information
  • Authors/Reviewers
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submit My Manuscript
  • Open Access
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Librarians
  • Institutional Subscriptions
  • Usage Stats
  • Support
  • Contact Us
  • Subscribe
  • Resources
  • Media Kit
  • About
  • International Access
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • FAQ
  • AAP.org
  • shopAAP
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
  • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
  • RSS
American Academy of Pediatrics

© 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics