Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
    • Supplements
    • Publish Supplement
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Differences in Infant and Parent Behaviors During Routine Bed Sharing Compared With Cot Sleeping in the Home Setting

Sally A. Baddock, Barbara C. Galland, David P.G. Bolton, Sheila M. Williams and Barry J. Taylor
Pediatrics May 2006, 117 (5) 1599-1607; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1636
Sally A. Baddock
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barbara C. Galland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David P.G. Bolton
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sheila M. Williams
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barry J. Taylor
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

OBJECTIVES. To observe the behavior of infants sleeping in the natural physical environment of home, comparing the 2 different sleep practices of bed sharing and cot sleeping quantifying to factors that have been identified as potential risks or benefits.

METHODS. Forty routine bed-sharing infants, aged 5–27 weeks were matched for age and season of study with 40 routine cot-sleeping infants. Overnight video and physiologic data of bed-share infants and cot-sleep infants were recorded in the infants' own homes. Sleep time, sleep position, movements, feeding, blanket height, parental checks, and time out of the bed or cot were logged.

RESULTS. The total sleep time was similar in both groups (bed-sharing median: 8.6 hours; cot-sleeping median: 8.2 hours). Bed-sharing infants spent most time in the side position (median: 5.7 hours, 66% of sleep time) and most commonly woke at the end of sleep in this position, whereas cot-sleeping infants most commonly slept supine (median: 7.5 hours, 100%) and woke at the end of sleep in the supine position. Prone sleep was uncommon in both groups. Head covering above the eyes occurred in 22 bed-sharing infants and 1 cot-sleeping infant. Five of these bed-sharing infants were head covered at final waking time, but the cot-sleeping infant was not. Bed-sharing parents looked at or touched their infant more often (median: 11 vs 4 times per night) but did not always fully wake to do so. Movement episodes were shorter in the bed-sharing group as was total movement time (37 vs 50 minutes respectively), whereas feeding was 3.7 times more frequent in the bed-sharing group than the cot-sleeping group.

CONCLUSIONS. Bed-share infants without known risk factors for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) experience increased maternal touching and looking, increased breastfeeding, and faster and more frequent maternal responses. This high level of interaction is unlikely to occur if maternal arousal is impaired, for example, by alcohol or overtiredness. Increased head covering and side sleep position occur during bed-sharing, but whether these factors increase the risk of SIDS, as they do in cot sleeping, requires further investigation.

  • cosleeping
  • SIDS
  • breastfeeding
  • sleep

Many groups value bed sharing, whether as a traditional practice, a positive parenting choice, or a way of coping with the demands of an infant.1–3 These varied motivations lead to considerable heterogeneity with regard to the actual practices involved.2,4–6 The practice is relatively common in the United Kingdom7 and has become more common in Western countries in the last 10 years, for example, the United States,8 Norway,9 and the Netherlands.10 This is in part coincident with the promotion of breastfeeding.9 Many advantages have been documented, for example, increased breastfeeding,11,12 increased mother-infant interactions,12 and increased infant arousals.12 However, bed sharing has also been identified as a risk factor for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) in combination with maternal smoking,13–15 alcohol consumption,14,15 maternal overtiredness,14 excessive or soft bedding,16 bed sharing with someone other than parents,17 and younger infant age.14,15,18 There may also be separate risks associated with sleeping in an adult bed without adults, similar to those identified with sleeping in any unusual place.14 The recent policy statement of the American Academy of Pediatrics did not target these risk factors but recommended against bed sharing during sleep.19 There are a number of postulated mechanisms for this increased risk but little research to provide evidence for them in the bed-share situation.

There is a need to identify benefits and risks to the infant and parent(s) to understand the ways bed sharing could be made safer for all infants. The change away from the prone sleep position has been very successful in many cultures at reducing the SIDS rate,20 but changes to other potentially modifiable factors have met with limited success.15,21 It may be more realistic and of more benefit to families that value bed sharing to identify ways to make it safer rather than increase guilt about what is a common and, for many cultures, a valued child-care practice.

When trying to assess the risks or benefits of bed sharing, cot sleeping is often taken as the norm; however, within many cultures, bed sharing is the norm or has historically been so.22 It is important to identify normative data for bed sharing rather than treating the 2 environments as if they are the same. Three groups have published findings from observational studies of infants sleeping overnight in a bed-share situation.12,23,24 These studies have been in sleep laboratories with infants at low risk of SIDS using a crossover design so that infants act as their own controls. This, however, means that infants are asked to sleep nights in a situation that is not their usual practice. As far as we are aware, there are no published studies of overnight family behavior conducted in the home environment comparing bed sharing and infant cot sleep.

The aim of this study, therefore, was to observe and document the behavior of families sleeping in their home environment, comparing the 2 different sleep practices of bed sharing and cot sleeping. This was to identify the differences between groups in regard to sleep time, sleep position, movements, feeding, blanket height, and parental checks, which may contribute to the mechanisms underlying risks and benefits identified from epidemiologic data.

METHODS

Two groups of infants were studied: 40 bed-sharing infants and 40 cot-sleeping infants. The sleep practice criteria was that bed-sharing infants regularly slept in the parental bed for a minimum of 5 hours per night whereas cot-sleeping infants regularly slept in a cot or bassinette in the parental bedroom ≥5 hours per night. None of the infants reported prenatal or postnatal complications (questionnaire). Bed-sharing infants were recruited through local postnatal groups and media advertising. Cot-sleeping infants matched for age and season of study were recruited from the local maternity ward. Infants were aged 0–6 months with 13 infant pairs aged 0–12.9 weeks, 15 pairs aged 13–19.9 weeks, and 12 pairs aged 20–27 weeks. All of the infants were at >37 weeks' gestation (except 2 infants in each group who were 28 and 32 weeks' gestation). The age of the 4 premature infants was adjusted to be consistent with 40 weeks' gestation. There were 14 pairs of studies in the winter compared with 8 to 10 studies in each of the other seasons. The study was approved by the Southern Regional Health Authority Ethics Committee, New Zealand (protocol 97/04/036). Informed consent was obtained from the parent(s) of all of the infants studied.

Protocol

Infants were monitored over 2 consecutive nights in their own home. The first night involved video recording only, and the second involved video and physiologic recording. The physiologic recordings involved placement of electrodes for recording raw electrocardiogram, oxygen saturation and heart rate, abdominal and chest movements of respiratory pattern, nasal airflow, shin and rectal temperature, and CO2 near the infant's face. The details of these recordings have been described previously.25 Infants were set up and recordings started by the researchers. Families were then left unattended for the night. Recordings were turned off in the morning when the researchers returned. For the behavioral recordings, a small surveillance camera (CEC-C38, Panasonic, Osaka, Japan) was mounted on a stand above the bed so that the full width and the top third of the bed were in the field of view to allow recordings of the infant's movements and positioning and any infant/parent interactions. A small, handheld portable television was used as a monitor to ensure correct positioning. An infrared light source (Dennard [Fleet, United Kingdom] 12 volt. 880 Med 50) was mounted on the stand to reflect light off the ceiling on to the recording area. The camera was connected to an analog video recorder (Panasonic AG-TL700) set to “long play” that allowed 15 hours of recording on a 3-hour videotape.

Video Analysis

Analysis of the video data for sleep time, sleep position, movements, feeding, blanket height, and parental checks was based on observations on the second night, allowing synchronization with physiologic recordings. Custom-developed computer software was used to log all of the significant events into a database with time code for correlation with the physiologic readings. C Video software (Envisionology, San Francisco, CA) and a connecting cable were used to link the time counter from the video player with a key command on the computer. The database (File Maker Pro 2.0; Claris Corporation, Santa Clara, CA) was customized to provide a file for each major behavior category and subcategory. The start and finish times and code for each event were logged in the database using computer key commands. The video counter was calibrated with the real time digitized on the recording tape. Although tapes were recorded as long play, they were viewed at normal tape speed.

Off-line logging of data started from when the infant was asleep. Sleep was identified from the video and defined as starting after the infant was settled for 2 minutes. Start and stop times for behavioral categories listed here were logged into the database from this start time until the final waking of the infant in the morning. Subcategories for sleep position were: side, prone, and supine; for blanket height: below chin, chin to eyes, and above eyes; for parental checks: father look, father touch, mother look, and mother touch; for infant movements: small movement, posture change (trunk or gross body movement), response to parent (any infant movement that occurred after movement by the adjacent adult), feeding, and time out of the cot. Sleep and awake periods were identified from the video. If the infant awoke during sleep and returned to a settled state within 2 minutes, this period was included as sleep. Awakenings that lasted for >2 minutes were described as awake. Study time was defined from when the infant was first asleep, regardless of the presence of an adult, until the infant woke in the morning. Sleep time was the accumulation of the infant sleep periods during the study time. Sleep efficiency was expressed as the percentage of total sleep time/total study time.

Statistical Analyses

Based on studies of high-risk behavior in cot-sleeping infants,26 it was predicted that 50% of bed-sharing and 20% of cot-sleeping infants were likely to experience a potentially dangerous event. Two samples of 40, using the 5% level of significance, have 80% power to show this difference between groups.

Although bed-share and cot-sleep infants were matched for age and season of study, data for both members of 4 pairs were not available. Data were, therefore, analyzed as 2 groups, and regression analysis, adjusting for infant age and season, was used to take the matching into account. Medians and interquartile ranges are presented to describe the data. A Kruskal-Wallis test, Poisson or negative binomial regression, to account for the overdispersion in the data, or linear regression based on log transformation values were used to compare the 2 groups for the behavior variables. Results, where appropriate, are presented as the risk ratios and 95% confidence intervals.

RESULTS

As shown in Table 1, bed-sharing and cot-sleeping infants were comparable with regard to gestational age, birth weight, male:female ratio, age at study, and weight at study. All of the bed-sharing infants and 35 of 40 cot-sleeping infants were breastfed. The age of the mother and the proportion of mothers educated to tertiary level were similar between the groups. A small number in both groups were identified as Maori, indigenous New Zealanders. Maternal smoking was more common in the cot group (25%) compared with the bed-sharing group (8%). Maternal alcohol consumption was minimal in all, ranging from “rarely” to 3 glasses of wine or beer per week, with 17 of 40 bed sharers and 15 of 40 mothers of cot sleepers reporting no alcohol consumption during or after pregnancy. The practice of bed sharing was reported to be adopted by mothers because of factors such as the ease of breastfeeding, the provision of a close and secure environment for the infant, a more settled infant, and a natural environment.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1

Infant and Maternal Group Characteristics

Total Sleep Time and Sleep Efficiency

The total study time was similar between groups (bed sharing: median, 9.7 hours [interquartile range: 8.8–10.2 hours]; cot-sleeping: median, 9.0 [interquartile range: 8.7–10.2]). The total sleep time, as determined by video observation, was also similar (bed sharing: median, 8.6 hours [interquartile range: 7.8–9.4]; cot-sleeping: median, 8.2 [interquartile range: 7.4–9.0]). Consequently, sleep efficiency was similar between groups (bed sharing: median, 90.7% [interquartile range: 87.1–94.6]; cot-sleeping: median, 87.1 [interquartile range: 84.1–96.2]).

Infant Sleep Position

The time spent in each of the 3 sleep positions (as defined by the infant trunk position) varied between the 2 groups of infants and is shown in Table 2. Bed-sharing infants spent most time in the side position (median: 5.7 hours, 66% sleep time) whereas cot-sleeping infants most commonly slept supine (median: 7.5 hours, 100% sleep time). The median time spent prone was not significantly different. At the end of the final sleep period, a similar distribution of sleep positions was observed. Bed-sharing infants were most commonly on their side (side: 23 infants; supine: 13; prone: 2), whereas cot-sleeping infants were commonly supine (side: 4 infants; supine: 33; prone: 2). The pattern of prone sleep varied between the 2 groups: 5 bed-sharing infants (aged 7, 8, 10, 22, and 23 weeks) spent some time prone (3.0, 3.5, 2.3, 2.2, and 1.6 hours, respectively), and 2 cot-sleeping infants (aged 8 and 25 weeks) slept the entire night in the prone position (8.9 and 10.2 hours, respectively).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 2

Infant Sleep Position: Duration and Percentage of Sleep Time in Each Position

Blanket Height Relative to Infant

Results for blanket height are shown in Table 3. Infants in both groups spent most of the night sleeping with the blankets below the level of the chin (bed-sharing median: 7.1 hours [82% of sleep time]; cot-sleeping median: 8.1 hours [100% of sleep time]). Bed-sharing infants spent significantly more time than cot infants with the blankets partially over the face (to the eyes) or with blankets above the eyes. Head-covering events (ie, blankets above the eyes) occurred in 22 bed-sharing infants and 1 cot-sleeping infant. At final awakening time, 5 of these bed-share infants had their head covered. The last head-covering incident for the cot-sleep infant finished 4 hours before final waking.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 3

Blanket Height Relative to the Infant's Face: Duration and Percentage of Sleep Time at Different Blanket Heights

Parental Checks

When mothers in both groups checked their infant, it usually involved touching rather than just looking at the infant. Table 4 shows that there was no significant difference in the amount of time bed-sharing mothers spent checking their infant compared with mothers of cot-sleeping infants. Fathers/partners rarely checked the infants (data not shown), but when all of the looks and touches by both parents were combined, bed-sharing parents checked their infant a median of 11 times compared with 4 checks by the parents of the cot-sleeping infants (P < .0001). When considering individuals, there were extreme examples, for example, parents in 1 bed-sharing study checked their infant 53 times. These were predominantly brief touches by the mother. Observations indicated that the bed-sharing parents did not always wake fully to check their infant, and small patting movements, in what seemed to be drowsy sleep, were common.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 4

Parental Checking (looking and/or touching): Total Duration and Number of Checks for Bed-Sharing and Cot-Sleeping Infants

Infant Movements

Table 5 shows that the most common type of movement recorded in both groups through the night was posture change. Bed-sharing infants spent significantly less time in posture change movements compared with cot-sleeping infants (37 vs 50 minutes, respectively). However, the number of posture change records was similar for both groups, suggesting that individual periods of posture change movement through the night were shorter for bed-sharing infants. There were significantly fewer small movements (brief hand movements) by the bed-sharing infants, and they occurred for less total time, whereas responses to mother were more frequent and lasted for longer total time. Feeding was 3.7 times more frequent in the bed-sharing group than the cot-sleeping group.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 5

Infant Movements and Feeding Sessions: Total Duration and Number of Movement Records for Bed-Sharing and Cot-Sleeping Infants

DISCUSSION

This study clearly demonstrated different behaviors of both the infant and parents when comparing bed-sharing and cot-sleeping practices. Although the cot was usually immediately adjacent to the parents' bed, the presence of the infant in the adult bed for bed-share studies resulted in very different behaviors. The study showed that regular bed-share infants engaged in more feeding and more infant-mother interactions than cot-sleep infants, side sleeping position was more common during sleep and at final waking in bed-share infants, and prone sleeping position, although rare, occurred for short intervals in bed-share infants, whereas it lasted all night for 2 cot infants. Incidents where the bedding or clothing covered the infant's head were more common in the bed-sharing situation both during the night and on final waking.

Previous smaller studies comparing the same practices have been conducted mainly in the laboratory setting,23,27 although attempts have been made to make the environment as home-like as possible. These studies used a crossover design that showed that, for many behaviors, the largest difference was recorded for regular bed sharers on their bed-share night compared with regular cot-sleepers on their cot-sleep night.23,27 This emphasizes the importance of observing infants in their regular sleep arrangement, as in this study. The present study supports the observations from laboratory studies that bed-share infants engage in more feeding episodes and are checked by their mother more frequently than cot-sleeping infants.23,28–30 Mothers often identified ease of breastfeeding as a reason for bed sharing. Population studies also support an association between bed sharing and breastfeeding31 and an association with breastfeeding persisting to an older infant age.32 Several large epidemiologic studies have shown a small but significant protective effect of breastfeeding against SIDS;17,33–36 however, this has not been shown in some others.37,38

Concern has been raised regarding the possibility of accidental asphyxiation from mothers falling asleep breastfeeding while lying down.39 No instances were noted where the mother was in a position that might have resulted in mechanical obstruction of the airways, and no oxygen desaturation events <90% with head covered (data not given) or any increase in rectal temperature outside the reference range was observed.25

The reason we videoed 2 nights was to establish whether there was any difference in behavior because of “first-night effect” or the presence of the sensors on the infant. Using Bland-Altman plots,40 we found no significant difference on key behavioral indices, such as sleep time, number of infant movements per hour, feeds, and sleep position, suggesting that attachment of the sensors did not have a significant effect on sleep behavior.

Although there is no direct evidence that increased maternal checking reduces SIDS, mothers in this study and others2,23,41,42 report an emotional benefit from bed sharing, because they can easily check their infant. Mothers have also been observed to actively check and modify infant temperature by rearranging bedding.43 Room sharing compared with infant sleeping in a separate room is protective against SIDS14,15,44 and may be related to increased maternal checks. It is likely that the dramatic reduction in the multivariate relative risk for infants not sharing the room and prone from 16.99 (95% CI: 10.43–27.69) to 3.28 (95% CI: 2.06–5.23) for infants sharing the room and prone44 is explained by increased awareness and checking of infants while they are asleep. In our study, whereas bed-share parents checked their infant more often, many of these checks were brief, involving minimal disruption to the mother's sleep. These findings are confirmed by Mosko et al,45 who reported that total sleep time of mothers was not decreased on bed-share nights compared with infant cot-sleep nights and that maternal awakenings were for shorter duration on the bed-share nights. It is likely that greater arousal and more disruption to maternal sleep would be needed to check an infant in a cot, even if it was nearby.

The risks and/or benefits associated with increased waking have been debated. In adults and children, sleep fragmentation is associated with many negative effects, such as increasing the frequency and duration of obstructive sleep apnea46 and increasing the arousal threshold.47,48 However, none of these studies have been conducted on breastfeeding women. It is not known whether the multiple, brief, drowsy awakenings through the night during bed sharing would have more or less impact on the mother than the few, full awakenings required to attend to an infant in the cot. Interestingly, mothers in this study, as in others, report “increased sleep” as a reason to bed share, along with “having a more settled infant.” Studies investigating the effect of mild sleep deprivation on infants report a possible increased propensity to upper airway obstruction49,50 and changes in autonomic control of cardiac function.51 However, it is likely that experimentally induced sleep deprivation has different physiologic manifestations from infant-initiated awakenings through the night associated with breastfeeding.

The finding in this study that the side sleep position was the most common sleep position for bed-share infants is in agreement with Ball's findings.30 However, whereas bed-share infants had significant periods of side sleep in laboratory-based studies,23,52 the supine position was predominant. The increased instrumentation for recording electroencephalogram, electro-oculogram, and electromyogram in these 2 studies might have affected the position mothers placed their infant to sleep (Helen Ball, PhD, written communication, 2002). The side sleep position has been identified as increasing the risk of SIDS,15,53 reportedly associated with the tendency of side-sleep infants to roll prone.15 However, the evidence for this has been established from infants sleeping in a cot. There is no data to establish the risk of this position during bed-share sleep. In our study, 12 cot-sleep infants spent some time sleeping on their side, but none were observed to roll to the prone position. All 38 of the bed-share infants slept some time on their side, and 1 infant, aged 23 weeks, was observed to roll to the prone position when the mother moved away from the infant. A characteristic sleep position of mother and breastfed infant that seems to prevent rolling has been described: mother sleeping in a lateral position, facing the infant, with her knees drawn up under the infant feet and the mother's arm positioned above the infant's head.54,55 This was observed in our study, but it was not universal, despite all of our mothers being breastfeeders.

Head covering by blankets occurred more often in the bed-share group, a finding also observed by Ball (Helen Ball, PhD, written communication, 2002) but not reported by others. Young23 found no instances of head and body completely covered by bedding. This may reflect a difference in home monitoring compared with the sleep laboratory, where arguably parents are more relaxed and more likely to engage in usual practices. Whether this behavior places these infants at risk is another question. Being found with head covered has been reported in several studies as increasing the risk of SIDS (odds ratio: 12.5; 95% confidence interval: 6.47–24.1).15 Although head covering was common among the bed-share infants in this study, only a quarter of infants with head-covering episodes during the night ended up with head covered at the end of sleep. Bedding tended to be moved on and off infants more often during the natural course of sleep through the night. This may help explain why bed-share infants are found with the head covered at the end of sleep less often than cot-sleep infants.56 Infants in the present study often stayed (without significant movement) in the head-covered position for long periods of time (eg, 3.5 hours by 1 infant), suggesting they were not uncomfortable. Our previous studies suggest that the risk of significant rebreathing into bedding depends on the type and thickness of covering,57,58 as well as the ability of the infant to mount both a respiratory and arousal response. Because infants of smoking mothers may well be the infants least likely to respond to this stress,59–61 the large interaction between smoking and bed sharing noted in epidemiologic studies may be explained by poor responsiveness to this particular occurrence during bed-sharing sleep.

Bed-share infants in this study had a different pattern of movements than the cot infants. Although there were the same numbers of posture change episodes in both groups, episodes were shorter in the bed-share group, resulting in a highly significant, reduced total posture change time. The presence of the mother, often touching or cradling the infant during sleep, may also have brought rapid reassurance to the bed-share infant and reduced nonawake movement episodes and, consequently, reduced stress experienced by infants.

It would seem that the bed-share infants in this study were at low risk of SIDS, because there were very few maternal smokers, all of the infants in the bed-sharing group were breastfed, most mothers had some form of tertiary education, and families actively chose to bed share because of perceived advantages to themselves and their infant. The findings may be quite different in bed-sharing families where many SIDS risk factors are prevalent13,17,62 and breastfeeding is not common.

Although this study has identified potential hazards that may be encountered during bed sharing, for example, head covering, it has also identified many potential benefits, for example, increased parental checks. This was not a surprising finding, because in many societies around the world, bed sharing is the preferred sleep arrangement. It is only relatively recently that white societies have moved to a solitary sleep arrangement, where conditioning infants to sleep through the night without waking is a goal valued by society.63 However, there is a growing trend among whites to choose to bed share as a parenting style.7–9

This study has highlighted many factors that seem to be common to both bed-sharing and cot-sleep infants but in fact vary in important ways because of the different physical environments and the presence of adults. Thus, risk factors identified for infants sleeping in a cot, for example, side sleep, may not be directly applicable to bed-sharing infants and require investigation by epidemiologic studies using cases and controls in the bed-share environment. Secondly, the benefits of bed sharing, for example, increased maternal checking, breastfeeding, and faster and more frequent maternal responses, rely on the mother's ability to arouse, at least partially, and respond to the infant through the night. Mothers impaired, for example, by alcohol or extreme overtiredness, may not be able to respond appropriately, thus stressing the importance of a healthy, nonimpaired mother in the bed-share partnership.

Acknowledgments

We thank Charrissa Makowharemahihi and Amanda Phillips for research assistance, Christine Rimene for advice on cultural aspects, Paul Bennington and Gordon Yau for assistance with customising the database for video logging, and the families that participated in the study.

This study was supported by a grant from the Health Research Council of New Zealand.

Footnotes

    • Accepted October 17, 2005.
  • Address correspondence to Barry Taylor, MBChB, FRACP, Department of Women's and Children's Health, University of Otago, PO Box 913, Dunedin, New Zealand. E-mail: barry.taylor{at}stonebow.otago.ac.nz
  • The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

SIDS—sudden infant death syndrome

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    McKenna JJ, Mosko S. Evolution and infant sleep - an experimental study of infant-parent co-sleeping and its implications for SIDS. Acta Paediatr.1993;82 :31– 36
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    Baddock SA, Day HF, Rimene CR, Moala AF, Taylor BJ, Day RR. Bedsharing practices of different cultural groups. 6th SIDS International Conference. Auckland, New Zealand; 2000
  3. ↵
    Ball HL, Hooker E, Kelly PJ. Where will the baby sleep? Attitudes and practices of new and experienced parents regarding cosleeping with their newborn infants. Am Anthropol.1999;101 :143– 151
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    Tuohy PG, Smale P, Clements M. Ethnic differences in parent/infant co-sleeping practices in New Zealand. NZ Med J.1998;111 :364– 366
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. Latz S, Wolf AW, Lozoff B. Cosleeping in context: sleep practices and problems in young children in Japan and the United States. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1999;153 :339– 346
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Nelson EA, Chan PH. Child care practices and cot death in Hong Kong. NZ Med J.1996;109 :144– 146
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. ↵
    Blair PS, Ball HL. The prevalence and characteristics associated with parent-infant bed-sharing in England. Arch Dis Child.2004;89 :1106– 1110
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    Willinger M, Ko CW, Hoffman HJ, Kessler RC, Corwin MJ. National Infant Sleep Position Study. Trends in infant bed sharing in the United States, 1993–2000: the National Infant Sleep Position Study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.2003;157 :43– 49
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Arnestad M, Andersen M, Vege A, Rognum TO. Changes in the epidemiological pattern of sudden infant death syndrome in southeast Norway, 1984–1998: Implications for future prevention and research. Arch Dis Child.2001;85 :108– 115
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    De Jonge GA, Hoogenboezem J. Epidemiology of 25 years of crib death (sudden infant death syndrome) in the Netherlands; incidence of crib death and prevalence of risk factors in 1980–2004 [abstract]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd,2005;149 :1273– 1278
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    Ball, HL, Breastfeeding, bed-sharing and infant sleep. Birth,2003.30 :181– 188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    McKenna J, Mosko S, Richard C, et al. Experimental studies of infant-parent co-sleeping—mutual physiological and behavioral influences and their relevance to SIDS (Sudden Infant Death Syndrome). Early Hum Dev.1994;38 :187– 201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Scragg R, Mitchell EA, Taylor BJ, et al. Bed sharing, smoking, and alcohol in the sudden infant death syndrome. New Zealand Cot Death Study Group. BMJ.1993;307 :1312– 1318
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    Blair P, Fleming P, Smith I, et al. Babies sleeping with parents: Case-control study of factors influencing the risk of the Sudden Infant Death Syndrome. BMJ.1999;319 :1457– 1462
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Carpenter RG, Irgens LM, Blair PS, et al. Sudden unexplained infant death in 20 regions in Europe: case control study. Lancet.2004;363 :185– 191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Flick L, White DK, Vemulapalli C, et al. Sleep position and the use of soft bedding during bed sharing among African American infants at increased risk for sudden infant death syndrome. J Pediatr.2001;138 :338– 343
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Hauck FR, Herman SM, Donovan M, et al. Sleep environment and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome in an urban population: the Chicago Infant Mortality Study. Pediatrics.2003;111 :1207– 1214
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    Tappin D, Russell E, Brooke H. Bedsharing, roomsharing and sudden infant death syndrome in Scotland: a case-control study. J Pediatr.2005;147 :32– 37
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    American Academy of Pediatrics, Task Force on SIDS. The changing concept of Sudden Infant Death Syndrome: diagnostic coding shifts, controversies regarding the sleeping environment, and new variables to consider in reducing risk. Pediatrics.2005;116 :1245– 1255
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Ponsonby AL, Dwyer T, Cochrane J. Population trends in sudden infant death syndrome. Semin Perinat.2002;26 :296– 305
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    Mitchell EA, Tuohy PG, Brunt JM, et al. Risk factors for sudden infant death syndrome following the prevention campaign in New Zealand: a prospective study. Pediatrics.1997;100 :835– 840
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Barry H, Paxson LM. Infancy and early childhood: cross-cultural codes 2. Ethnology.1971;10 :466– 508
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    Young J. Night-Time Behaviour and Interactions Between Mothers and Their Infants of Low Risk for SIDS: A Longitudinal Study of Room-Sharing and Bedsharing [PhD thesis]. Bristol, United Kingdom: University of Bristol; 1999
  24. ↵
    Ball HL. Triadic bed-sharing and infant temperature. Child Care Health Dev.2002;28 :55– 58
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Baddock SA, Galland BC, Beckers MGS, Taylor BJ, Bolton DPG. Bedsharing and the infant's thermal environment in the home setting. Arch Dis Child.2004;89 :1111– 1116
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Waters KA, Gonzalez A, Jean C, Morielli A, Brouillette RT. Face-straight-down and face-near-straight-down positions in healthy, prone-sleeping infants. J Pediatrs.1996;128 :616– 625
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. ↵
    Richard CA, Mosko SS, McKenna JJ. Apnea and periodic breathing in bed-sharing and solitary sleeping infants. J Appl Physiol.1998;84 :1374– 1380
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    McKenna JJ, Mosko SS, Richard CA. Bedsharing promotes breastfeeding. Pediatrics.1997;100 :214– 219
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. Pollard K, Fleming P, Young J, Sawczenko A, Blair P. Night-time non-nutritive sucking in infants aged 1 to 5 months: relationship with infant state, breastfeeding, and bed-sharing versus room-sharing. Early Hum Dev.1999;56 :185– 204
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Ball HL. Parent-infant bed-sharing behaviour: effects of feeding type, and presence of father. Human Nature.2006; In press
  31. ↵
    McCoy RC, Hunt CE, Lesko SM, et al. Frequency of bed sharing and its relationship to breastfeeding. J Devel Behav Pediatr.2004;25 :141– 149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Vogel A, Hutchison BL, Mitchell EA. Factors associated with the duration of breastfeeding. Acta Paediatr.1999;88 :1320– 1326
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Hoffman HJ, Hillman LS. Epidemiology of the sudden infant death syndrome: maternal, neonatal, and postneonatal risk factors. Clin Perinatol.1992;19 :717– 737
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. Ford RP, Taylor BJ, Mitchell EA, et al. Breastfeeding and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome. Int J Epidemiol.1993;22 :885– 890
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. Andersen M, Arnestad M, Rognum TO, Vege A. Crib death in the eastern regions of Norway 1984–1992. A survey of risk factors. Tidssk Nor Laegeforen.1995;115 :34– 37
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    McVea KL, Turner PD, Peppler DK. The role of breastfeeding in sudden infant death syndrome. J Hum Lact.2000;16 :13– 20
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Ponsonby AL, Dwyer T, Kasl SV, Cochrane JA. The Tasmanian SIDS Case-Control Study: Univariable and multivariable risk factor analysis. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol.1995;9 :256– 272
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    Fleming PJ, Blair PS, Bacon C, et al. Environment of infants during sleep and risk of the sudden infant death syndrome: results of 1993–5 case-control study for confidential inquiry into stillbirths and deaths in infancy. CESDI Regional Coordinators and Researchers. BMJ.1996;313 :191– 195
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Byard RW. Is breast feeding in bed always a safe practice? J Paediatr Child Health.1998;34 :418– 419
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet.1986;1 :307– 310
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Abel S, Park J, Tipene-Leach D, Finau S, Lennan M. Infant care practices in New Zealand: a cross-cultural qualitative study. Soc Sci Med.2001;53 :1135– 1148
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Ball HL. Reasons to bed-share: why parents sleep with their infants. J Reprod Infant Psychol.2002;20:207–222
  43. ↵
    Sawczenko A, Galland BC, Young J, Ring W, Fleming PJ. Night time mother- infant interactive behaviour and physiology: a longitudinal comparison of room sharing versus bedsharing (“co-sleeping”). Pediatr Pulmonol.1995;20 :341
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    Scragg RK, Mitchell EA, Stewart AW, et al. Infant room-sharing and prone sleep position in sudden infant death syndrome. New Zealand Cot Death Study Group. Lancet.1996;347 :7– 12
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Mosko S, Richard C, McKenna J. Maternal sleep and arousals during bedsharing with infants. J Sleep Res Sleep Med.1997;20 :142– 150
    OpenUrl
  46. ↵
    Guilleminault C. Sleep apnea syndromes: impact of sleep and sleep states. Sleep.1980;3 :227– 234
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    Roehrs T, Merlotti L, Petrucelli N, Stepanski E, Roth T. Experimental sleep fragmentation. Sleep.1994;17 :438– 443
    OpenUrlPubMed
  48. ↵
    Ferrara M, De Gennaro L, Casagrande M, Bertini A. Auditory arousal thresholds after selective slow-wave sleep deprivation. Clin Neurophysiol.1999;110 :2148– 2152
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Kahn A, Groswasser J, Sottiaux M, Rebuffat E, Franco P. Mechanisms of obstructive sleep apneas in infants. Biol Neonate.1994;65 :235– 239
    OpenUrlPubMed
  50. ↵
    Thomas DA, Poole K, McArdle EK, et al. The effect of sleep deprivation on sleep states, breathing events, peripheral chemoresponsiveness and arousal propensity in healthy 3 month old infants. Eur Respir J.1996;9 :932– 938
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    Franco P, Seret N, Van Hees JN, Lanquart JP Jr, Groswasser J, Kahn A. Cardiac changes during sleep in sleep-deprived infants. Sleep.2003;26 :845– 848
    OpenUrlPubMed
  52. ↵
    Richard C, Mosko S, McKenna J, Drummond S. Sleeping position, orientation, and proximity in bedsharing infants and mothers. Sleep.1996;19 :685– 690
    OpenUrlPubMed
  53. ↵
    Skadberg BT, Markestad T. Infant behaviour in response to a change in body position from side to prone during sleep. Eur J Pediatr.1996;155 :1052– 1056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Mosko S, Richard C, McKenna J, Drummond S, Mukai D. Maternal proximity and infant CO2 environment during bedsharing and possible implications for SIDS research. Am J Phys Anthropol.1997;103 :315– 328
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    Ball HL. Differences in bed-sharing behaviour among breastfeeding and non-breastfeeding families. 7th SIDS International Conference. Florence, Italy; 2002
  56. ↵
    Blair P, Fleming D, Smith IJ, Ward Platt M. Are risk factors for SIDS infants found sharing the parental bed different from those found in the cot? 7th SIDS International Conference. Florence, Italy; 2002
  57. ↵
    Campbell AJ, Bolton DPG, Williams SM, Taylor BJ. A potential danger of bedclothes covering the face. Acta Paediatr.1996;85 :281– 284
    OpenUrlPubMed
  58. ↵
    Campbell AJ, Taylor BJ, Bolton DPG. Comparison of two methods of determining asphyxial potential of infant bedding. J Pediatr.1997;130 :245– 249
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Lewis KW, Bosque EM. Deficient hypoxia awakening response in infants of smoking mothers: possible relationship to sudden infant death syndrome. J Pediatr.1995;127 :691– 699
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. Campbell AJ, Galland BC, Bolton DPG, Taylor BJ, Sayers RM, Williams SM. Ventilatory responses to rebreathing in infants exposed to maternal smoking. Acta Paediatr.2001;90 :793– 800
    OpenUrlPubMed
  61. ↵
    Hafstrom O, Milerad J, Asokan N, Poole SD, Sundell HW. Nicotine delays arousal during hypoxemia in lambs. Pediatr Res.2000;47 :646– 652
    OpenUrlPubMed
  62. ↵
    Brenner RA, Simons-Morton BG, Bhaskar B, Revenis M, Das A, Clemens JD. Infant-parent bed sharing in an inner-city population. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.2003;157 :33– 39
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Morelli GA, Rogoff B, Oppenheim D, Goldsmith D. Cultural variation in infants' sleeping arrangements: questions of independence. Dev Psychol.1992;28 :604– 613
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  • Copyright © 2006 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
PreviousNext
Back to top

Advertising Disclaimer »

In this issue

Pediatrics
Vol. 117, Issue 5
May 2006
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
View this article with LENS
PreviousNext
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Differences in Infant and Parent Behaviors During Routine Bed Sharing Compared With Cot Sleeping in the Home Setting
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Request Permissions
Article Alerts
Log in
You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Differences in Infant and Parent Behaviors During Routine Bed Sharing Compared With Cot Sleeping in the Home Setting
Sally A. Baddock, Barbara C. Galland, David P.G. Bolton, Sheila M. Williams, Barry J. Taylor
Pediatrics May 2006, 117 (5) 1599-1607; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-1636

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Differences in Infant and Parent Behaviors During Routine Bed Sharing Compared With Cot Sleeping in the Home Setting
Sally A. Baddock, Barbara C. Galland, David P.G. Bolton, Sheila M. Williams, Barry J. Taylor
Pediatrics May 2006, 117 (5) 1599-1607; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-1636
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Print
Download PDF
Insight Alerts
  • Table of Contents

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Wahakura Versus Bassinet for Safe Infant Sleep: A Randomized Trial
  • SIDS and Other Sleep-Related Infant Deaths: Evidence Base for 2016 Updated Recommendations for a Safe Infant Sleeping Environment
  • SIDS and Other Sleep-Related Infant Deaths: Evidence Base for 2016 Updated Recommendations for a Safe Infant Sleeping Environment
  • Hypoxic and Hypercapnic Events in Young Infants During Bed-sharing
  • SIDS and Other Sleep-Related Infant Deaths: Expansion of Recommendations for a Safe Infant Sleeping Environment
  • Head covering - a major modifiable risk factor for sudden infant death syndrome: a systematic review
  • Sleep Arrangements and Behavior of Bed-Sharing Families in the Home Setting
  • Sleep Environment, Positional, Lifestyle, and Demographic Characteristics Associated With Bed Sharing in Sudden Infant Death Syndrome Cases: A Population-Based Study
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Neonatal SARS-CoV-2 Infections in Breastfeeding Mothers
  • Racial and Ethnic Diversity in Studies Funded Under the Best Pharmaceuticals for Children Act
  • Clinical Impact of a Diagnostic Gastrointestinal Panel in Children
Show more Articles

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Fetus/Newborn Infant
    • Fetus/Newborn Infant
    • SIDS
  • Nutrition
    • Breastfeeding
  • Journal Info
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Policies
  • Overview
  • Licensing Information
  • Authors/Reviewers
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submit My Manuscript
  • Open Access
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Librarians
  • Institutional Subscriptions
  • Usage Stats
  • Support
  • Contact Us
  • Subscribe
  • Resources
  • Media Kit
  • About
  • International Access
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • FAQ
  • AAP.org
  • shopAAP
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
  • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
  • RSS
American Academy of Pediatrics

© 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics