Skip to main content

Advertising Disclaimer »

Main menu

  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers
  • Other Publications
    • American Academy of Pediatrics

User menu

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
American Academy of Pediatrics

AAP Gateway

Advanced Search

AAP Logo

  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
  • Journals
    • Pediatrics
    • Hospital Pediatrics
    • Pediatrics in Review
    • NeoReviews
    • AAP Grand Rounds
    • AAP News
  • Authors/Reviewers
    • Submit Manuscript
    • Author Guidelines
    • Reviewer Guidelines
    • Open Access
    • Editorial Policies
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Online First
    • Archive
    • Blogs
    • Topic/Program Collections
    • AAP Meeting Abstracts
  • Pediatric Collections
    • COVID-19
    • Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health
    • More Collections...
  • AAP Policy
  • Supplements
  • Multimedia
    • Video Abstracts
    • Pediatrics On Call Podcast
  • Subscribe
  • Alerts
  • Careers

Discover Pediatric Collections on COVID-19 and Racism and Its Effects on Pediatric Health

American Academy of Pediatrics
Article

Breastfeeding Is Analgesic in Healthy Newborns

Larry Gray, Lisa W. Miller, Barbara L. Philipp and Elliott M. Blass
Pediatrics April 2002, 109 (4) 590-593; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.109.4.590
Larry Gray
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lisa W. Miller
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barbara L. Philipp
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elliott M. Blass
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments
Loading
Download PDF

Abstract

Context. This study identifies a behavioral and nonpharmacologic means of preventing newborn pain.

Objective. To determine whether breastfeeding is analgesic in newborn infants undergoing heel lance—a routine, painful, hospital procedure.

Design. A prospective, randomized, controlled trial.

Setting. Hospital maternity services at Boston Medical Center, Boston, Massachusetts, and Beverly Hospital, Beverly, Massachusetts.

Participants. A random sample of 30 full-term, breastfed infants.

Interventions. Infants in the intervention group were held and breastfed by their mothers during heel lance and blood collection procedures for the Newborn Screening Program Blood Test. Infants in the control group experienced the same blood test while receiving the standard hospital care of being swaddled in their bassinets.

Outcome Measures. Crying, grimacing, and heart rate differences were analyzed between the breastfeeding and the control infants before, during, and after blood collection.

Results. Crying and grimacing were reduced by 91% and 84%, respectively, from control infant levels during the blood collection. Heart rate was also substantially reduced by breastfeeding.

Conclusions. Breastfeeding is a potent analgesic intervention in newborns during a standard blood collection.

  • breastfeeding
  • skin-to-skin contact
  • crying
  • grimacing
  • heart rate
  • heel lance
  • human newborns
  • pain

Breastfeeding links evolutionary biology and medical practice. This is of clinical interest because pain is routinely experienced in hospital settings, even by healthy newborns, and natural interventions are effective at a time when many pharmacologic interventions are not.1 Premature and sick infants often experience multiple painful diagnostic and therapeutic procedures for which the long-term consequences are not known.2 Single, intense, painful experiences such as circumcision remain manifest for at least months in the form of exaggerated reactivity to routine immunizations.3,4 In animal models, intermittent pain can permanently affect limbic catecholamine levels and reactions to situational or pharmacologic stressors.5 The claim can no longer be made that newborn pain is for the moment only.

Recent studies have demonstrated that certain tastes and flavors alleviate newborn pain. As little as 2 mL of milk,6,7 its fat,6 protein6 components, or sweet substances8–25 all reduce pain in human and rat infants and eliminate spontaneous crying as well. Moreover, in rats the mechanisms underlying these taste-induced analgesics are opioid-mediated26–28 and block pain afferents at the level of the spinal cord.29

Nonnutritive suckling itself is also “antinociceptive” in rat and human infants,8,30 and suckling experience is not required for its manifestation.31 Contact alone, in the absence of suckling, also dulls pain reactivity. Specifically, crying and grimacing are markedly reduced during blood collection in newborns held by their mothers in full-body contact. Such contact also blocks the substantial increase in heart rate that normally accompanies blood collection.32 Although the mechanisms underlying tactile-induced “antinociception” have not been identified, they do not seem to be opioid-mediated.33

The purpose of this study was to unite the different components of nursing (taste, suckling, and skin-to-skin contact), which have been shown to be individually analgesic, by allowing newborns to suckle their nursing mothers before, during, and after a standard heel lance procedure for blood collection. The efficacy of this intervention was determined by evaluating video recordings of infant crying and facial expressions and by assessing blockade of heart rate increases that normally accompany the blood collection procedure.

METHODS

Participants

The study participants were 30 healthy, full-term, breastfed newborns delivered at Boston Medical Center, Boston, Massachusetts, and Beverly Hospital, Beverly, Massachusetts, between January and November 1999. After mothers had given written, informed consent, their infants were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 study groups using a system of sealed envelopes. Thirty envelopes, 15 breastfeeding and 15 control, each designating group assignment, were mixed and shuffled. After informed consent was obtained, the top envelope from the stack was opened identifying the group assignment. In the intervention group (N = 15), infants were breastfed during blood collection. Control infants (N = 15) were swaddled in their bassinets during the procedure (standard hospital treatment). One infant failed to complete the study because of difficulties with latching at the breast. This infant’s group assignment was resealed in an envelope and reshuffled with the remaining envelopes. The 2 groups were demographically equal as determined by 2-tailed Fisher exact tests or Student t tests (Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
TABLE 1.

Characteristics of Study Groups

All infants had been delivered by normal, spontaneous, vaginal delivery, and no infant had presented any evidence of congenital abnormalities, medical complications, or drug exposure. No infant had required either oxygen administration or ventilatory support. Infants had been fed between 30 minutes and 4 hours before the start of the study. The average time between last feed and the heel lance procedure was 198 minutes for the breastfeeding group and 197.5 minutes for the control group. The difference between these 2 groups did not differ by a t test comparison (P > .05). This was the initial heel lance for all infants, and no infant had been circumcised before the procedure.

Based on our previous study,32 in which skin-to-skin contact provided antinociception during heel lance, we calculated that a sample size of 15 infants per group was necessary to achieve a statistically reliable reduction in grimacing and crying, with a power of 80% and a P value < .05. The institutional review boards of the Boston Medical Center, Boston University School of Medicine, University of Massachusetts at Amherst, and Beverly Hospital approved the protocol governing these studies. Seventy-eight percent of the mothers approached for participation gave their consent.

Data Collection

All infants were prepared for the study by placing 3 safety electrodes (Klear Trace 2000-S; CAS Medical Systems, Branford, CT) on their thoracic region and a warming pad on 1 heel. Control infants were then wrapped in their receiving blankets and placed on their side in their crib. Infants in the breastfeeding group, wearing only a diaper, were returned to their mothers to initiate breastfeeding. Mothers were reclined and cradled their infants during breastfeeding to maintain full-body, skin-to-skin contact during the entire procedure. The study was timed to coincide with the next feed. A lactation consultant (L.W.M.) assisted with positioning and assessment of suckling. The study was initiated when the infant maintained a good latch as determined by a large amount of areola in the mouth, flanged lips, and active jaw movement. This generally required 30 to 60 seconds to achieve. Once this was achieved, infant and mother were covered with 2 receiving blankets in a manner that left the infant’s face visible for filming from the side of the bed. The mother’s face did not appear on film.

The study proper began with filming (Sony Model CCD-TRV32, New York, NY) the infant’s face during breastfeeding or during rest (control infants). Heart rates were announced every 10 seconds from the monitor. After the 2-minute baseline period, the heel warmer was removed, and the heel was swabbed with alcohol. Then, to reduce procedural variability, 1 physician (L.G.) used a spring-loaded lancing device (Tenderfoot, International Technidyne Corporation, Edison, NJ) to draw blood for the Newborn Screening Program Blood Test. After blood collection was completed, a Band-Aid (Johnson & Johnson Consumer Products Company, New Brunswick, NJ) was applied to the heel. This initiated the recovery phase of the study, which lasted an additional 2 minutes. The entire study lasted about 7 to 8 minutes.

Because L.G. conducted all heel lances, a potential bias of differential treatment has been introduced. Having only 1 person conduct the procedure markedly reduces experimenter variability. We are not concerned about bias because procedure duration between experimental and control groups did not differ (t test comparison; P = .8298). Moreover, heel lance procedure time duration and amount of cry in the present study are highly comparable with infants in our earlier studies,8,32 thereby making it less likely that experimenter bias determined present group differences.

Outcome Measures

Videotapes were scored by research assistants, who were neither informed of study design nor aware of the number of study groups. Facial grimacing was scored continuously from the video portion of the tape, and crying and heart rate was scored from the audio portion with video blank, thereby resulting in scorers who were uninformed about which group an infant belonged to during heart rate and crying scoring. Crying was scored continuously as the presence of an audible crying sound independent of quality. Facial grimacing was scored when brow bulging, eye squeezing, and nasolabial folding occurred simultaneously; the 3 measures determined most sensitive for pain by the studies of Grunau and Craig.34–36 Very occasionally, the breast partially obscured the infant’s face. In these instances, 2 of the 3 pain markers were required to be present to score a facial grimace. Heart rate was scored in 10-second intervals from the audio portions of the videotape. Because the changes in heart rate are large and sustained, we feel justified in using the current index. The heart rate was continuously monitored, and in our experience once the circa 25 beat per minute (bpm) heart rate increase had occurred, there is little moment-to-moment heart rate variability. Finally, intercoder reliability was 95% for all measures.

Statistical Evaluations

Simple t tests were performed on the mean percent cry and grimace measures during blood collection. Heart rate changes for the 2 groups were assessed through a 2-factor analysis of variance with repeated measures across the 3 phases of the experiment (baseline, blood collection, and recovery intervals). All analyses used SAS software (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC).

RESULTS

The outcome of this study is clear: breastfeeding during blood collection essentially eliminated crying and grimacing. Breastfeeding also prevented the marked rise in heart rate that normally accompanies heel lance.

Breastfeeding infants cried for only 4% (P = .0002; standard deviation [SD]: 7.23%; mean duration: 8.77 seconds) and grimaced for 8% (P = .0001; SD: 12.18%; mean duration: 17.25 seconds) of the entire blood collection procedure compared with 43% crying (SD: 30.83%; mean duration: 72.07 seconds) and 50% grimacing (SD: 26.69%; mean duration: 80.31 seconds) in control infants (Fig 1). In fact, 11 of these 15 breastfeeding infants did not cry or grimace at all during this otherwise painful experience. These effects extended well into the recovery phase. Only 1 of the 15 infants in the breastfeeding group cried at all during recovery. He did so for a total of 10 seconds. In contrast, the mean duration of crying during recovery for infants in the control group was 28 seconds.

Fig 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 1.

Mean (+ SEM) percentage occurrence of grimacing and crying during the heel stick procedure for control newborns and newborns who were breastfeeding before and during the procedure (N = 15/group).

Breastfeeding also prevented the tachycardia induced by blood collection (Fig 2). Overall, a significant interaction between group and procedure time was observed (P = .001) indicating that the change in mean heart rate across the precollection, collection, and recovery intervals differed between breastfed and control infants during and after heel lance. In particular, mean increase in heart rate between baseline and blood collection phases for control infants was 29 bpm, whereas that for nursing infants was only 6 bpm.

Fig 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig 2.

Mean heart rate (bpm) at 10-second intervals for the entire study of control and breastfeeding infants.

DISCUSSION

Breastfeeding before, during, and after a painful blood collection procedure markedly suppressed crying and grimacing and prevented the increase in heart rate that normally accompanies this procedure. Indeed, 11 of the 15 breastfeeding infants did not cry or grimace at all during the procedure. When watching these infants on videotape, we could not tell when blood collection was either initiated or terminated. We believe that this reflects pain blockade, as opposed to suckling producing a behavior that is incompatible with crying. Infants could detach from the nipple, and some did. In any event, suckling certainly does not interfere with expressions in facial countenance that convey pain perception.

The nursing-induced analgesia documented herein and its component parts of taste-, suckling-, and contact-induced analgesias are all available to newborns at term and probably before, judged by the widespread success of sucrose-induced analgesia in premature infants. The present data do not allow us to evaluate which component of the suckling act contributed most to the analgesia, nor do they allow assessment of interaction among components. The most available comparison is with the contact-induced analgesia. Skin-to-skin contact was effective in reducing the pain experienced during heel lance. Eight of 15 infants held by their mothers during the procedure did not cry at all, and their crying was reduced 83% compared with their controls. Three infants in skin-to-skin contact did not grimace at all during the heel lance, and grimacing was reduced 65% compared with their controls.32 Breastfeeding caused 11 of 15 infants not cry at all during the heel lance procedure (9 of 15 did not even grimace) and reduced mean cry levels by 90% and grimacing by 84% compared with their controls. Although formal statistical comparisons are precluded across studies, we believe it is unlikely that the analgesic effect of breastfeeding can be exclusively attributed to skin-to-skin contact because of the somewhat greater amount of grimacing seen in the skin-to-skin procedure. Moreover, our clinical experience with these 2 interventions leads us to favor breastfeeding. Simply holding the infant while preventing access to the breast frustrated the mother and infant. Uniformly, a period of time—as much as 15 minutes for some—was required for the 2 to relax and settle down. In contrast, infants immediately initiated breastfeeding and the heel lance began within seconds after the infant had established a good suck.

The extensive parallel findings between rat and human infants suggest that the components of these analgesias are both phylogenetically conserved and robust. Ren et al29 have demonstrated that analgesia in suckling rats that received sucrose, reflected blockade of spinal afferents at the level of the dorsal horn. Parallel findings between the species in behaviorally induced analgesias, and the lack of any indication of discomfort in 11 of the suckling infants, makes the dorsal horn a reasonable point of impact in the present study as well. (See also Taddio3 on this point).

A comment on our control group is necessary. The use of a “standard hospital treatment” control group is simultaneously a strength and limitation of this study. The ability to compare the analgesic properties of breastfeeding with standard care reveals breastfeeding’s robust effect. This behaviorally induced analgesia and the similarly effective procedure of skin-to-skin contact32 and orosensory stimulation6–22 for alleviating pain during routine heel lance add to the growing scientific literature and international consensus37 of the necessity to provide appropriate analgesia for newborn pain. As such, the continued use of “no treatment” controls is becoming ethically difficult to defend with numerous safe and effective pain-relieving options available. Indeed, our own institution Boston Medical Center in reflecting the progress of our discipline now requires that parents be offered safe, natural, ways to combat newborn pain. These options include breastfeeding and tasting sucrose for routine painful procedures.

Finally, the present findings must be placed in a context broader than relieving immediate newborn pain or stress. The rapidly expanding literature about the subtle stresses experienced by animal and human newborns convincingly demonstrates that even mild stress during this early period can cause permanent changes in limbic catecholamine neurotransmission and exaggerated behavioral and endocrinologic reactivity to mild adult stress. The American Academy of Pediatrics promotes breastfeeding for proven “health, nutritional, immunologic, developmental, psychological, social, economic and environmental benefits.”38 The present findings now add the benefits of analgesia and stress reduction. It would seem prudent to exploit evolutionary-conserved systems in the medical management of stress and pain during early infancy, the time when many otherwise potent pharmacological interventions are not appropriate.1

Acknowledgments

This research has been supported by grant RO-1 MH51705–04A1 and Research Scientist Award KO-5, MH5 00 524 from the National Institutes of Mental Health to Dr Blass. Dr Gray was a Fellow in Behavioral and Developmental Pediatrics at Boston University School of Medicine and Boston Medical Center, and was supported by a Maternal and Child Health Bureau Training grant MCJ-259169. Presently, he is in the Section of Behavioral and Developmental Pediatrics at the University of Chicago and La Rabida Children’s Hospitals.

We thank the nursing staffs at Boston Medical Center and Beverly Hospital for their cooperation and help in conducting this research, as well as the parents who allowed us to study their infants.

bpm, beats per minute • SD, standard deviation

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    Blass EM, Barr RG. Evolutionary biology and the practice of medicine: the case of management of infant pain experience [in process citation]. J Dev Behav Pediatr.2000;21 :283– 284
    OpenUrlPubMed
  2. ↵
    American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Fetus and Newborn and Committee on Drugs. Prevention and management of pain and stress in the neonate. Pediatrics.2000;105 :454– 461
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Taddio A, Katz J, Ilersich AL, Koren G. Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain response during subsequent routine vaccination. Lancet.1997;349 :599– 603
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Taddio A, Goldbach M, Ipp M, Stevens B, Koren G. Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain responses during vaccination in boys. Lancet.1995;345 :291– 292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Kehoe P, Clash K, Skipsey K, Shoemaker WJ. Brain dopamine response in isolated 10-day-old rats: assessment using D2 binding and dopamine turnover. Pharmacol Biochem Behav.1996;53 :41– 49
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Blass EM. Milk-induced hypoalgesia in human newborns. Pediatrics.1997;99 :825– 829
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    Blass EM. Infant formula quiets crying human newborns. J Dev Behav Pediatr.1997;18 :162– 165
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Blass EM, Watt LB. Suckling- and sucrose-induced analgesia in human newborns. Pain.1999;83 :611– 623
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. Johnston CC, Stremler RL, Stevens BJ, Horton LJ. Effectiveness of oral sucrose and simulated rocking on pain response in preterm neonates. Pain.1997;72 :193– 199
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. Barr RG, Pantel MS, Young SN, Wright JH, Hendricks LA, Gravel R. The response of crying newborns to sucrose: is it a “sweetness” effect? Physiol Behav.1999;66 :409– 417
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. Barr RG, Quek VS, Cousineau D, Oberlander TF, Brian JA, Young SN. Effects of intra-oral sucrose on crying, mouthing and hand-mouth contact in newborn and six-week-old infants. Dev Med Child Neurol.1994;36 :608– 618
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. Blass EM, Hoffmeyer LB. Sucrose as an analgesic for newborn infants. Pediatrics.1991;87 :215– 218
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. Blass EM, Shah A. Pain-reducing properties of sucrose in human newborns. Chem Senses.1995;20 :29– 35
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. Haouari N, Wood C, Griffiths G, Levene M. The analgesic effect of sucrose in full term infants: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ.1995;310 :1498– 1500
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. Bucher HU, Moser T, von Siebenthal K, Keel M, Wolf M, Duc G. Sucrose reduces pain reaction to heel lancing in preterm infants: a placebo-controlled, randomized and masked study. Pediatr Res.1995;38 :332– 335
    OpenUrlPubMed
  16. Stevens B, Ohlsson A. Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures. Cochrane Database Syst Rev.2000:2
  17. Skogsdal Y, Eriksson M, Schollin J. Analgesia in newborns given oral glucose. Acta Paediatr.1997;86 :217– 220
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. Ramenghi LA, Wood CM, Griffith GC, Levene MI. Reduction of pain response in premature infants using intraoral sucrose. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed.1996;74 :F126– F128
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. Abad F, Diaz NM, Domenech E, Robayna M, Rico J. Oral sweet solution reduces pain-related behaviour in preterm infants. Acta Paediatr.1996;85 :854– 858
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. Ramenghi LA, Griffith GC, Wood CM, Levene MI. Effect of non-sucrose sweet tasting solution on neonatal heel prick responses. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed.1996;74 :F129– F131
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. Stevens B, Taddio A, Ohlsson A, Einarson T. The efficacy of sucrose for relieving procedural pain in neonates–a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr.1997;86 :837– 842
    OpenUrlPubMed
  22. ↵
    Herschel M, Khoshnood B, Ellman C, Maydew N, Mittendorf R. Neonatal circumcision. Randomized trial of a sucrose pacifier for pain control [published erratum appears in Arch Pediatr Adolesc. 1998;152:448]. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.1998;152 :279– 284
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. Blass EM, Ciaramitaro V. A new look at some old mechanisms in human newborns: taste and tactile determinants of state, affect, and action. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev.1994;59 :1– 81
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. Smith BA, Stevens K, Torgerson WS, Kim JH. Diminished reactivity of postmature infants to sucrose compared with term infants. Dev Psychol.1996;28 :811– 820
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    Smith BA, Fillion TJ, Blass EM. Orally-mediated sources of calming in one- to three- day-old human infants. Dev Psychol.1996;26 :731– 737
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    Blass EM, Fitzgerald E. Milk-induced analgesia and comforting in 10-day-old rats: opioid mediatrion. Pharmacol Biochem Behav.1988;29 :9– 13
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. Blass E, Fitzgerald E, Kehoe P. Interactions between sucrose, pain and isolation distress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav.1987;26 :483– 489
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Shide DJ, Blass EM. Opioidlike effects of intraoral infusions of corn oil and polycose on stress reactions in 10-day-old rats. Behav Neurosci.1989;103 :1168– 1175
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Ren K, Blass EM, Zhou Q, Dubner R. Suckling and sucrose ingestion suppress persistent hyperalgesia and spinal Fos expression after forepaw inflammation in infant rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.1997;94 :1471– 1475
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    Campos RG. Rocking and pacifiers: two comforting interventions for heelstick pain. Res Nurs Health.1994;17 :321– 331
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    Blass EM, Jackson AM, Smotherman WP. Milk-induced, opioid-mediated antinociception in rats at the time of cesarean delivery. Behav Neurosci.1991;105 :677– 686
    OpenUrlPubMed
  32. ↵
    Gray L, Watt L, Blass EM. Skin-to-skin contact is analgesic in healthy newborns. Pediatrics.2000;105(1) . Available at: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/105/1/e14
  33. ↵
    Blass EM, Fillion TJ, Weller A, Brunson L. Separation of opioid from nonopioid mediation of affect in neonatal rats: nonopioid mechanisms mediate maternal contact influences. Behav Neurosci.1990;104 :625– 636
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Hadjistavropoulos HD, Craig KD, Grunau RV, Johnston CC. Judging pain in newborns: facial and cry determinants. J Pediatr Psychol.1994;19 :485– 491
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. Grunau RV, Johnston CC, Craig KD. Neonatal facial and cry responses to invasive and non-invasive procedures. Pain.1990;42 :295– 305
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Grunau RV, Craig KD. Pain expression in neonates: facial action and cry. Pain.1987;28 :395– 410
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Anand KJ. Consensus statement for the prevention and management of pain in the newborn. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.2001;155 :173– 10
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    American Academy of Pediatrics, Work Group on Breastfeeding. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Breastfeed Rev.1998;6 :31– 36
    OpenUrlPubMed
  • Copyright © 2002 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
PreviousNext
Back to top

Advertising Disclaimer »

In this issue

Pediatrics
Vol. 109, Issue 4
1 Apr 2002
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
View this article with LENS
PreviousNext
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Academy of Pediatrics.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Breastfeeding Is Analgesic in Healthy Newborns
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Academy of Pediatrics
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Academy of Pediatrics web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Request Permissions
Article Alerts
Log in
You will be redirected to aap.org to login or to create your account.
Or Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Breastfeeding Is Analgesic in Healthy Newborns
Larry Gray, Lisa W. Miller, Barbara L. Philipp, Elliott M. Blass
Pediatrics Apr 2002, 109 (4) 590-593; DOI: 10.1542/peds.109.4.590

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Breastfeeding Is Analgesic in Healthy Newborns
Larry Gray, Lisa W. Miller, Barbara L. Philipp, Elliott M. Blass
Pediatrics Apr 2002, 109 (4) 590-593; DOI: 10.1542/peds.109.4.590
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Print
Download PDF
Insight Alerts
  • Table of Contents

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgments
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • Comments

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Infant Analgesia With a Combination of Breast Milk, Glucose, or Maternal Holding
  • Safe Sleep and Skin-to-Skin Care in the Neonatal Period for Healthy Term Newborns
  • Sucrose and Warmth for Analgesia in Healthy Newborns: An RCT
  • Pain Outcomes in a US Children's Hospital: A Prospective Cross-Sectional Survey
  • Analgesia with breastfeeding in addition to skin-to-skin contact during heel prick
  • Skin-to-Skin Contact Diminishes Pain From Hepatitis B Vaccine Injection in Healthy Full-Term Neonates
  • Ergonomic Procedure for Heel Sticks and Shots in Kangaroo Care (Skin-to-Skin) Position
  • Relief of Pain and Anxiety in Pediatric Patients in Emergency Medical Systems
  • Effective Analgesia Using Physical Interventions for Infant Immunizations
  • Analgesic Effect of Breast Milk Versus Sucrose for Analgesia During Heel Lance in Late Preterm Infants
  • Considerations for Using Sucrose to Reduce Procedural Pain in Preterm Infants
  • Behavioral Approaches to Anxiety and Pain Management for Pediatric Venous Access
  • Breastfeeding or Oral Sucrose Solution in Term Neonates Receiving Heel Lance: A Randomized, Controlled Trial
  • Pain Reduction During Pediatric Immunizations: Evidence-Based Review and Recommendations
  • Effects of Breast Milk on the Severity and Outcome of Neonatal Abstinence Syndrome Among Infants of Drug-Dependent Mothers
  • Breastfeeding and the Use of Human Milk
  • Relief of Pain and Anxiety in Pediatric Patients in Emergency Medical Systems
  • Assessment and management of pain in infants
  • Methadone and Breastfeeding: New Horizons
  • Analgesic effect of breast feeding in term neonates: randomised controlled trial
  • Breast-Feeding Reduces Pain
  • Breast-Feeding Is Analgesic in Newborns
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Evaluation of an Emergency Department High-risk Bruising Screening Protocol
  • National Perinatal Hepatitis B Prevention Program: 2009–2017
  • Time to First Onset of Chest Binding–Related Symptoms in Transgender Youth
Show more Article

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Nutrition
    • Nutrition
    • Breastfeeding
  • Journal Info
  • Editorial Board
  • Editorial Policies
  • Overview
  • Licensing Information
  • Authors/Reviewers
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submit My Manuscript
  • Open Access
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Librarians
  • Institutional Subscriptions
  • Usage Stats
  • Support
  • Contact Us
  • Subscribe
  • Resources
  • Media Kit
  • About
  • International Access
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • FAQ
  • AAP.org
  • shopAAP
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Instagram
  • Visit American Academy of Pediatrics on Facebook
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Twitter
  • Follow American Academy of Pediatrics on Youtube
  • RSS
American Academy of Pediatrics

© 2021 American Academy of Pediatrics