In-School Neurofeedback Training for ADHD: Sustained Improvements From a Randomized Control Trial

Abstract
OBJECTIVE: To evaluate sustained improvements 6 months after a 40-session, in-school computer attention training intervention using neurofeedback or cognitive training (CT) administered to 7- to 11-year-olds with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
METHODS: One hundred four children were randomly assigned to receive neurofeedback, CT, or a control condition and were evaluated 6 months postintervention. A 3-point growth model assessed change over time across the conditions on the Conners 3–Parent Assessment Report (Conners 3-P), the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Parent Form (BRIEF), and a systematic double-blinded classroom observation (Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools). Analysis of variance assessed community-initiated changes in stimulant medication.
RESULTS: Parent response rates were 90% at the 6-month follow-up. Six months postintervention, neurofeedback participants maintained significant gains on Conners 3-P (Inattention effect size [ES] = 0.34, Executive Functioning ES = 0.25, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity ES = 0.23) and BRIEF subscales including the Global Executive Composite (ES = 0.31), which remained significantly greater than gains found among children in CT and control conditions. Children in the CT condition showed delayed improvement over immediate postintervention ratings only on Conners 3-P Executive Functioning (ES = 0.18) and 2 BRIEF subscales. At the 6-month follow-up, neurofeedback participants maintained the same stimulant medication dosage, whereas participants in both CT and control conditions showed statistically and clinically significant increases (9 mg [P = .002] and 13 mg [P < .001], respectively).
CONCLUSIONS: Neurofeedback participants made more prompt and greater improvements in ADHD symptoms, which were sustained at the 6-month follow-up, than did CT participants or those in the control group. This finding suggests that neurofeedback is a promising attention training treatment for children with ADHD.
- ADHD —
- attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
- BOSS —
- Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools
- BRIEF —
- Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function
- CompAT —
- computer attention training
- Conners 3-P —
- Conners 3–Parent Assessment Report
- CT —
- cognitive training
- RA —
- research assistant
What’s Known on This Subject:
An estimated 9.5% of children are diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which affects academic and social outcomes. We previously found significant improvements in ADHD symptoms immediately after neurofeedback training at school.
What This Study Adds:
This randomized controlled trial included a large sample of elementary school students with ADHD who received in-school computer attention training with neurofeedback or cognitive training. Students who received neurofeedback were reported to have fewer ADHD symptoms 6 months after the intervention.
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with core symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, and/or impulsivity and has a prevalence of 9.5% for 4- to 17-year-olds in the United States. 1 Executive functioning is typically impaired in children with ADHD, affecting their academic achievement.2 Medication and behavior therapy are both viable treatment options for ADHD,3 but they both have limitations. These limitations, along with the pervasiveness of ADHD symptoms in school, highlight the importance of researching alternative treatments that can be implemented in the classroom setting. Computer attention training (CompAT) is an umbrella term used to describe many computer interventions that appear to be effective4 and that might be possible to implement on a large scale in school. Based on theories of operant conditioning and brain plasticity, the goal of CompAT interventions is to decrease ADHD symptoms and improve executive functioning skills. CompAT interventions may provide sustainable benefits even after the intervention is terminated through its conditioning and generalization components. Two types of CompAT interventions were evaluated in the current study: neurofeedback and cognitive training (CT).
EEG patterns in children with ADHD have shown more theta wave activity and increased theta:beta ratio in the frontal cortex, compared with children without ADHD.5–7 Beta Waves in the frontal cortex are associated with sustaining attention and thinking, whereas theta waves are prevalent when drowsy or daydreaming. However, other studies have not confirmed the finding that children with ADHD have elevated theta:beta ratios when compared with controls.8,9 The authors of these studies hypothesized that children in control conditions also have elevated theta:beta ratios than has been observed in the past, potentially due to decreased sleep (among other factors), making the 2 groups look more alike. When training attention, neurofeedback provides children with immediate auditory and visual feedback regarding their level of attention during each exercise. Changes are enabled because of brain plasticity of the frontal brain, which continues to develop throughout childhood and into early adulthood.10 Neurofeedback therefore trains users to monitor and change their brainwave patterns, leading to behavioral changes.11 Some studies have found that neurofeedback can decrease symptoms of ADHD,12–17 including improved attention,18 behavior,19 and cognitive improvements20 up to 6 months postintervention as well as at 2 years postintervention. 21 However, the evidence for its sustainability remains unclear, because there are limited studies examining follow-up data, and those that do have small sample sizes or no control condition.13–15
In contrast, CT uses specifically designed exercises to train attention, working memory, and impulsivity through ongoing feedback to reinforce correct responses. Several studies suggest that CT improves performance on working memory tasks and decreases inattentiveness, hyperactivity, and disruptive behaviors.22–26 The largest such trial included only 44 children diagnosed with ADHD, ages 7 to 12 years, and reported results 3 months after completing a 20-session intervention.26
Gevensleben et al18 examined neurofeedback and CT after 6 months and found that improvements in the neurofeedback condition on parent-reported behavior scales were significantly superior and sustained compared with the CT condition. Unfortunately, significant attrition makes this study’s generalizability unclear. A recent meta-analysis regarding nonpharmacologic interventions for ADHD concluded that increased evidence is needed for both neurofeedback and CT interventions before they can be supported as treatments for ADHD.27
The current study is novel for several reasons. The research team conducted the first in-school translational efficacy trial comparing neurofeedback, CT, and control conditions. Previous studies have mostly been conducted in laboratories or in clinical settings. This efficacy trial targeted a precise age range of children 7 to 11 years of age, as opposed to previous studies that included diverse developmental age ranges. Many studies are smaller without a control group and failed to find group differences. Last, very few studies reported follow-up results.
Pre- to postintervention, we found significantly greater improvements in ADHD symptoms, including attention and executive functioning, among neurofeedback participants compared with the control and CT conditions.28 In the present article, we report outcomes 6 months after the conclusion of the intervention. We hypothesized that participants receiving neurofeedback would maintain improvements in attention and executive functioning compared with control or CT conditions and that medication dosage would remain stable.
Methods
Participants
Students with ADHD who were attending 1 of 19 public elementary suburban or urban schools in the Greater Boston area were eligible to participate in the randomized trial. Inclusion criteria included the following: (1) child in second or fourth grade, (2) clinical diagnosis of ADHD made by the child’s clinician, and (3) ability to speak and understand English well enough to follow the protocol, although English was not necessarily the participant’s first language. Exclusion criteria included (1) a coexisting diagnosis of conduct disorder, autism spectrum disorder, or other serious mental illness (eg, psychosis) and (2) an IQ measured by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test <80, to limit confounding factors and requirements of extensive amendments to the intervention protocol that could affect standardized implementation. The study was located in schools, and investigators had no clinical responsibility for the children’s medical care. Therefore, children were included on the basis of their clinician’s diagnosis of ADHD, and were included regardless of whether they were taking medications for ADHD. Parents of all participants were informed that they should continue to adhere to scheduled clinician visits and standard community treatments (including counseling and medication management) independent of study participation, and medication use was not suspended for treatments or assessments. The study was approved by the Tufts Medical Center Institutional Review Board, and written informed consent and child assent were obtained.
Enrollment of the first cohort occurred from May to September 2009 and from May to September 2010 for the second cohort. All preintervention assessments were conducted in October, and interventions were initiated in November of each year. For each cohort, the research coordinator balanced participants on the basis of school district, gender, and medication status, and then assigned them via a computer random number generator into 3 conditions (neurofeedback, CT, and control). Before enrollment, parents were told their child would be randomly assigned into 1 of these 3 conditions, and were informed of their child’s group status after assignments were made.
Interventions
Participants received in-school 45-minute intervention sessions 3 times per week, monitored by a trained research assistant (RA), for 40 sessions over 5 months. The same protocol was used for both intervention conditions. RAs received a standardized 2-week training to administer neurofeedback and CT, followed by a posttraining test and direct observation assessments. RAs filled out a standardized session checklist for each child at every session to monitor implementation fidelity.
The specific neurofeedback system used (Play Attention, Unique Logic and Technology, Fletcher, NC) detects 2 frequency ranges, 1 in the low-frequency theta brainwave range (4–8 Hz) and another in the high-frequency beta brainwave range (12–15 Hz).29 The brainwaves are measured by an EEG sensor embedded in a standard bicycle helmet centrally located on the top of the skull, and 2 other EEG sensors one a grounding sensor and the other a reference, on the chin straps located bilaterally on the mastoids. Through practice, participants learn to manipulate the figures on the screen, resulting in suppression of theta and an increase in beta activity. As the theta:beta ratio changes, an algorithm is used so that participants score points on the computer program and learn how to improve attention on the 6 different exercises.
The specific CT intervention used (Captain’s Log, BrainTrain, North Chesterfield, VA) comprises exercises that train different areas of cognition, which may be designed into personalized exercise protocols. The system is well designed for large-scale delivery, because there is automatic level advancement after each exercise.30 The standardized protocol developed for this study is composed of 14 auditory and visual exercises targeting areas of attention and working memory. Each exercise is interactive and lasts ∼5 minutes. Both systems are commercially available.
Primary Outcome Measures
Outcome measures included parent reports of ADHD symptoms and executive functioning, medication use, and systematic classroom observations of behavior. All outcome measures were obtained pre- and postintervention, and 6 months later.
The Conners 3–Parent Assessment Report (Conners 3-P; Multi-Health Systems Inc, North Tonawanda, NY) is a validated and standardized instrument to assess ADHD symptoms,31 including 9 subscales comprising 2 summary scales summed together as a Global Index. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) (PAR Inc, Lutz, FL) is a validated and standardized instrument that assesses executive functioning,32 including 8 subscales comprising 2 indices summed together in the Global Executive Composite. Both parents, if available, completed the Conners 3-P and BRIEF.
The Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools (BOSS; Pearson Education, Inc, New York, NY) 33 is a systematic interval recording observation system for coding classroom behavior and reports on engagement (active or passive) and off-task behaviors (motor, verbal, and passive). Data output from observations are objective quantitative assessments, which can help reduce observer bias, and consist of raw data as well as the percentage of intervals the participant was recorded as engaged or off-task. The BOSS has been found to be reliable between observers, 34 to differentiate between children with ADHD and their typically developing peers, 35 and to be sensitive to treatment effects. 36 The BOSS was completed 3 times at each time point (ie, before the intervention, immediately after the intervention, and 6 months after the intervention) for all study participants by trained RAs37 who were unaware of the participants’ randomization conditions. The participants were unaware that they were being observed.
A Medication Tracking Questionnaire was completed by the primary parent at each time point to track medication type, dosage, and history. No direct consultation regarding medication was discussed with parents, who were encouraged to continue their regularly scheduled visits with their clinician. Stimulant medications were converted into methylphenidate equivalencies by the research team to compare dosage over time. The reliability of parent reports was assessed by comparing name and dosages of medication at each time point. Ambiguous responses were clarified by direct communication with parents and clinicians.
Data Analysis
Analysis of variance was conducted to assess baseline differences in demographic data between randomization conditions. Missing items within multiitem scales were resolved by using expectation maximization imputation,38 which is an iterative imputation method suitable for low-frequency missing data and/or when SEs are not of primary concern. 39 When a full questionnaire was missing, it was dropped from the analysis and addressed directly through the analytic strategy described below. Because this study investigated whether the 2 CompAT interventions are superior to community treatment alone, and whether neurofeedback is superior to CT, this randomized controlled trial is considered a superiority trial and analyses are presented with 1-tailed tests.40–42
The central focus of these analyses was to evaluate whether the observed changes in core ADHD symptoms between the start and end of the treatment period were sustained at the 6-month follow-up. Changes in parent-reported and classroom observation measures were investigated by 3-point growth models by using a multilevel approach to assess change over the 3 time points (preintervention, postintervention, and 6-month follow-up) to compare neurofeedback and CT with the control.43–45 Our approach used all available data, including the reports from 2 parents when available at all 3 time points. These models allow for the estimation of reliability of measurement and change within the overall estimation, and can flexibly accommodate unbalanced data, so a participant can be included at a time point even if only 1 parent questionnaire was available at any or all of the time points. For the BOSS, 3 observations at all 3 time points were used to estimate reliability.46 This linear model estimates the best-fitting line to the 3 time points. Comparisons between neurofeedback and CT were undertaken using multivariate general linear hypothesis tests.47 For ease in interpretation and comparison with other studies, approximate effects sizes (expressed as standardized mean differences, Cohen’s d) were computed from the neurofeedback and CT coefficients from the growth models; however, to the best of our knowledge, no other study of CompAT reports growth coefficients and, furthermore, standard calculations do not accommodate all of the parameters estimated in a multilevel model.48 All growth models were estimated by using HLM version 7.0.42 All other analyses and data treatment were conducted by using SYSTAT version 13.0.49
Paired t tests were conducted to evaluate stimulant medication differences in methylphenidate equivalencies within randomization conditions between preintervention and the 6-month follow-up. An analysis of covariance was conducted to evaluate medication dosage differences among the randomization conditions at 6-month follow-up, controlling for preintervention stimulant medication dosages.
Results
Of the 104 children in the study, 102 completed the intervention. Of these, only 4 did not complete the 6-month follow-up assessment (n = 98) (Fig 1). The mean response rates of the parent questionnaires for pre- and postintervention data were 94% for the primary parent and 77% for the secondary parent. At the 6-month follow-up, response rates were 90% for the primary parent and 82% for the secondary parent. The BOSS was completed 3 times for each participant at preintervention, postintervention, and 6-month follow-up for 100% of participants. At baseline, 95% of participants showed clinically significant scores ≥65 on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, ADHD Inattention and/or ADHD Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales. At baseline, 49% of participants were taking medication. There were no statistically significant differences between randomization conditions at baseline with regard to gender, family income, race, medication use, or baseline ADHD symptoms (Table 1). There were no significant differences between participants who completed or who did not complete the intervention, or between randomization conditions at 6-month follow-up regarding gender, family income, or race. There were no adverse side effects in neurofeedback or CT interventions reported on the session checklists.
CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) diagram. a In a small number of cases, parent or teacher data were missing; therefore, sample sizes may be somewhat smaller than is indicated here.
Participant Characteristics
Growth Model Analysis
The majority of distributions for the measures at each time point and the changes were approximately symmetrical and tailed, but normality could not be assumed for all scales, so we relied on the robust SEs available in HLM42 in the assessment of hypotheses in the Conners 3-P, BRIEF, and BOSS models. The slopes of the primary scales of research interest on the Conners 3-P, BRIEF, and BOSS are displayed to show change over time by condition.
Parent-Reported Measures
Participants in the neurofeedback condition showed significant improvements over time compared with the control condition on Conners 3-P in the intervention-targeted areas of inattention, executive functioning, and hyperactivity/impulsivity as well as in 4 of 6 general behavior subscales (Table 2 and Supplemental Table 4) and on all 3 BRIEF summary index scales as well as 7 of 8 BRIEF subscales (Table 2 and Supplemental Table 5). Participants in the CT condition showed significant improvements over time compared with the control on only 1 of the 5 Conners 3-P subscales (Table 2) and on 2 of 8 BRIEF subscales (Supplemental Table 5). Furthermore, participants in the neurofeedback condition showed significant improvements over time compared with the CT condition on 6 Conners 3-P subscales (Supplemental Table 4) and on 6 BRIEF subscales (Supplemental Table 5). See Fig 2 for observed participant mean scores across the 3 study time points by condition in core ADHD and executive functioning areas.
Primary Measures: Parent Results
Observed participant mean scores across 3 study time points. NF, neurofeedback.
Classroom Observation
Results from the linear growth model did not show sustained change; however, the linear model was not a good fit for Off-task Motor/Verbal, therefore a quadratic model was estimated and significant improvements were found in the neurofeedback condition compared with the control (P = .04). There were no differences found between neurofeedback and CT conditions on classroom observation measures (Table 3).
BOSS Results
Medication Analysis
Among participants receiving stimulant medication, the mean dosage change in the neurofeedback condition from preintervention to 6-month follow-up was a 0.70-mg methylphenidate-equivalent increase (P = .44). In both CT and control conditions, parents reported significant increases: 13.08 mg for CT (P = .02) and 9.14 mg for the control (P < .001). No between-group dosage difference was found at 6-month follow-up, controlling for preintervention (P = .08).
Discussion
The outcomes of these analyses are promising. Parents of children in the neurofeedback condition reported sustained improvements 6 months after the intervention, compared with those in the control condition. In the CT condition, areas of executive functioning that did not show statistically significant change immediately after the intervention showed a significant change by the 6-month follow-up assessment compared with the control condition. Even after the intervention had stopped, parents continued to notice improvements in response to both interventions. Although similar to the Arns et al12 meta-analysis, improvements seen in the hyperactivity/impulsivity-related scales in the neurofeedback condition are surprising, because hyperactivity was not directly targeted in the intervention. Nevertheless, these findings suggest that when children’s focus increases, physical activity level is reduced.
Clinician’s management of medication was conducted independently of the study protocol. It is noteworthy that participants in the neurofeedback condition showed maintenance of stimulant medication dosage while presumably experiencing the same physical growth and increased school demands as CT and control condition peers, whose medication dosage increased clinically and statistically (9- to 13-mg methylphenidate-equivalent units).
This study used multiple sources and types of data including questionnaires from parents, systematic classroom observations of behavior, and medication. Because children had a different teacher at pre- and postintervention compared with the 6-month follow-up, teacher reports were not included in these analyses. The inclusion of the systematic classroom observations provided a valid double-blinded representation of the children’s behavior in the classroom.
Randomization of subjects to treatment conditions, as applied in this study, is the gold standard for clinical trials. Even though stratified by gender, school system, and medication status and well balanced regarding demographic characteristics across all 3 randomized conditions, the participants in the 3 conditions appeared to differ in the severity of baseline ADHD symptoms. However, none of these differences reached significance, and it is unclear how these differences in baseline severity might have affected the results. Furthermore, we relied on growth models to isolate change over time, not status at posttreatment or follow-up; our time coding, which centered time at posttreatment, was selected to reduce the correlation of initial status and change.
Parents were aware of the type of intervention their child received, which was unavoidable, because 1 of the systems uses a helmet and the other does not. Parents were informed that the 2 interventions were both commercially available and had achieved similarly encouraging results in previous studies at the time of enrollment. At postintervention, we found no differences in satisfaction with the intervention between parents with participants in the neurofeedback condition and parents with participants in the CT condition, suggesting that parent bias most likely did not affect their reporting of the measures.
Conclusions
Neurofeedback participants showed significant improvements that were sustained 6 months after the intervention compared with those in the control and CT conditions, as reported by the parents consistently on all of the core ADHD subscales and executive functioning scales. Participants in the CT condition showed significant improvement 6 months after the intervention period on 2 executive functioning subscales. Medication dosage was sustained among participants in the neurofeedback condition, whereas for CT and control conditions it was increased. The finding that neurofeedback was superior to CT on multiple scales further supports its efficacy as a treatment of children with ADHD. Effects were reported earlier in the neurofeedback condition than in the CT condition and were also stronger at the 6-month follow-up period, showing the promise of neurofeedback as a treatment with sustained gains for children with ADHD.
This is the first large randomized controlled trial to evaluate the long-term efficacy of in-school CompAT. Despite the paucity of scientific data, both neurofeedback and CT training systems are currently being used in school systems across the United States,29,30 underlining the importance of systematic studies of their effectiveness. The direct impact of attention deficits on academic progress makes schools an ideal setting for such an intervention, because all children with ADHD in all communities could potentially have access to these services on an ongoing basis. A next important step will be to assess individual participant differences to evaluate which factors might be associated with the most progress on the respective interventions and to study older developmental age cohorts.
Acknowledgments
We thank Tahnee Sidhu and Katie Tomasetti from Tufts Medical Center for their extensive contributions to this research project. We appreciate the assistance of Dr David Gotthelf, PhD, of the Newton Public Schools, Principal Simon Ho and Zhen Su of the Boston Public Schools, and the administrators and teachers of both school systems. Dr R. Chris Sheldrick, PhD, provided wise advice from the beginning of the project. We also acknowledge the following former RAs affiliated with Tufts Medical Center for their hard work on this study: Susan Mangan, Minakshi Ratkalkar, Lauren Rubin, Wendy Si, Melissa Arbar, Stefanie Moynihan, Neena Schultz, Elizabeta Bourchtein, Kolleen Burbank, Heather Bentley, Amanda Civiletto, Joyce Kao, and Jessica Charles, as well as students Cathryn Magielnicki and Lisa Ngu from Tufts University and Jessica Bennett and Jessica Chen from Northeastern University. We also acknowledge all of the participants and their families.
Footnotes
- Accepted December 18, 2013.
- Address correspondence to Naomi J. Steiner, MD, Floating Hospital for Children at Tufts Medical Center, 800 Washington St, Box 334, Boston, MA 02111. E-mail: nsteiner{at}tuftsmedicalcenter.org
Dr Steiner conceptualized and designed the study, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. Ms Frenette and Ms Rene carried out the initial analyses, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. Dr Brennan carried out the growth model analyses, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted.
This trial has been registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (identifier NCT01583829).
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: All phases of this study were supported by an Institute of Education Sciences grant (R305A090100).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- 1.↵
- Visser SN,
- Bitsko RH,
- Danielson ML,
- Perou R,
- Blumberg SJ,
- et al
- 2.↵
- Biederman J,
- Monuteaux MC,
- Doyle AE,
- et al
- 3.↵
- The MTA Cooperative Group.
- 4.↵
- Steiner NJ,
- Sheldrick RC,
- Gotthelf D,
- Perrin EC
- 5.↵Loo SK, Barkley RA. Clinical utility of EEG in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Appl Neuropsychol. 2005;12(2):64–76
- 6.
- Loo SK,
- Makeig S
- 7.↵
- 8.↵
- Liechti MD,
- Valko L,
- Müller UC,
- et al
- 9.↵
- Arns M,
- Conners CK,
- Kraemer HC
- 10.↵Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron. 2010;67(5):728–734
- 11.↵Heinrich H, Gevensleben H, Strehl U. Annotation: neurofeedback—train your brain to train behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(1):3–16
- 12.↵
- Arns M,
- de Ridder S,
- Strehl U,
- Breteler M,
- Coenen A
- 13.↵
- Moriyama TS,
- Polanczyk G,
- Caye A,
- Banaschewski T,
- Brandeis D,
- Rohde LA
- 14.
- 15.↵
- Hodgson K,
- Hutchinson AD,
- Denson L
- 16.
- Gevensleben H,
- Holl B,
- Albrecht B,
- et al
- 17.↵Williams JM. Does neurofeedback help reduce attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? J Neurother. 2010;14(4):261–279
- 18.↵
- Gevensleben H,
- Holl B,
- Albrecht B,
- et al
- 19.↵Strehl U, Leins U, Goth G, Klinger C, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N. Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: a new treatment for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006;118(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/118/5/e1530
- 20.↵
- 21.↵Gani C, Birbaumer N, Strehl U. Long term effects after feedback of slow cortical potentials and of theta-beta amplitudes in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Int J Bioelectromagn. 2008;10(4):209–232
- 22.↵
- 23.
- Holmes J,
- Gathercole SE,
- Place M,
- Dunning DL,
- Hilton KA,
- Elliott JG
- 24.
- 25.
- Monastra VJ,
- Lynn S,
- Linden M,
- Lubar JF,
- Gruzelier J,
- LaVaque TJ
- 26.↵
- Klingberg T,
- Fernell E,
- Olesen PJ,
- et al
- 27.↵
- Sonuga-Barke EJS,
- Brandeis D,
- Cortese S,
- et al.,
- European ADHD Guidelines Group
- 28.↵
- Steiner NS,
- Frenette EC,
- Rene KR,
- Brennan RT,
- Perrin EC
- 29.↵Play Attention [home page on the Internet]. Available at: www.playattention.com. Accessed August 22, 2013
- 30.↵BrainTrain [home page on the Internet]. Available at: www.braintrain.com. Accessed August 21, 2013
- 31.↵
- Conners CK,
- Wells KC,
- Parker JD,
- Sitarenios G,
- Diamond JM,
- Powell JW
- 32.↵
- Mahone EM,
- Cirino PT,
- Cutting LE,
- et al
- 33.↵Shapiro ES. Academic Skills Problems. 4th Ed. Workbook. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2011
- 34.↵Swanson JM. School-Based Assessments and Interventions for ADD Students. Irvine, CA: KC Publishing; 1992
- 35.↵
- DuPaul GJ,
- Volpe RJ,
- Jitendra AK,
- Lutz JG,
- Lorah KS,
- Gruber R
- 36.↵
- Volpe R,
- DiPerna J,
- Hintze J,
- Shapiro E.
- 37.↵Steiner NJ, Sidhu TK, Rene KM, Tomasetti KM, Frenette EC, Brennan RT. Development and testing of a direct observation code training protocol for elementary aged students with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Educ Asse Eval Acc. DOI 10.1007/s11092-013-9166-x
- 38.↵
- Dempster AP,
- Laird NM,
- Rubin DB
- 39.↵
- 40.↵
- Sackett DL
- 41.
- Piaggio G,
- Elbourne DR,
- Altman DG,
- Pocock SJ,
- Evans SJ,
- CONSORT Group
- 42.↵
- Raudenbush S,
- Bryk A,
- Congdon R
- 43.↵
- Goldberg AE,
- Sayer A
- 44.
- 45.↵
- 46.↵
- Raudenbush SW,
- Brennan RT,
- Barnett RC
- 47.↵
- Raudenbush SW,
- Bryk AS
- 48.↵Wilson DB. Effect size calculator. Available at: www.campbellcollaboration.org/escalc/html/EffectSizeCalculator-Home.php. Accessed August 22, 2013
- 49.↵
- Systat Software, Inc
- Copyright © 2014 by the American Academy of Pediatrics